2013
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12080
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

First Possession, History, and Young Children's Ownership Judgments

Abstract: It is impossible to perceive who owns an object; this must be inferred. One way that children make such inferences is through a first possession bias--when two agents each use an object, children judge the object belongs to the one who used it first. Two experiments show that this bias does not result from children directly inferring ownership from first possession; the experiments instead support an alternative account according to which the first possession bias reflects children's historical reasoning. In E… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

5
60
2

Year Published

2013
2013
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 61 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
(31 reference statements)
5
60
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Crucially, the agent acted without directly contacting the target-instead the agent threw some thing at it. For example, in some scenarios the agent threw a rock at a can, crushing 1 This claim is also supported by developmental studies show that children consider objects history in their ownership judgments (e.g., Friedman, Van de Vondervoort, Defeyter, & Neary, 2013;Gelman, Manczak, & Noles, 2012;Gelman, Noles, & Stilwell, 2014;Nancekivell & Friedman, 2014). 2 Our discussion of possession principally concerns the claim that taking physical possession of a non-owned object establishes ownership over it.…”
Section: The Current Approachmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Crucially, the agent acted without directly contacting the target-instead the agent threw some thing at it. For example, in some scenarios the agent threw a rock at a can, crushing 1 This claim is also supported by developmental studies show that children consider objects history in their ownership judgments (e.g., Friedman, Van de Vondervoort, Defeyter, & Neary, 2013;Gelman, Manczak, & Noles, 2012;Gelman, Noles, & Stilwell, 2014;Nancekivell & Friedman, 2014). 2 Our discussion of possession principally concerns the claim that taking physical possession of a non-owned object establishes ownership over it.…”
Section: The Current Approachmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…For example, when possession is pitted against testimony about ownership, 3‐ and 4‐year‐olds base ownership judgments on testimony, suggesting that they view testimony as the more definitive source of information (Blake et al., ; Neary & Friedman, in press, Experiment 2B). Likewise, first possession is trumped by strong age and gender stereotypes, and by information about an object's history (Friedman et al., ; Malcolm et al., in press). But it remains unknown whether children appreciate the definitiveness of causally relevant information about how an object was acquired.…”
Section: Who Owns What?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, people may require experience with museums, economic markets, and/or the signaling value of luxury objects in order to value object features that are neither obvious nor functionally relevant. In contrast, others have proposed that high evaluation of unique objects follows from a foundational, early-emerging belief that objects are imbued with their history (Friedman, Vondervoort, Defeyter, & Neary, 2013; Gelman, 2013; Nancekivell & Friedman, 2014; Newman, in press). On this view, even young children should care about an object's past when evaluating its desirability.…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%