1988
DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.917
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Finding the face in the crowd: An anger superiority effect.

Abstract: Facial gestures have been given an increasingly critical role in models of emotion. The biological significance of interindividual transmission of emotional signals is a pivotal assumption for placing the face in a central position in these models. This assumption invited a logical corollary, examined in this article: Face-processing should be highly efficient. Three experiments documented an asymmetry in the processing of emotionally discrepant faces embedded in crowds. The results suggested that threatening … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

68
863
25
14

Year Published

1989
1989
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1,045 publications
(983 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
68
863
25
14
Order By: Relevance
“…Although we point to the classic literature on preparedness for detecting angry faces as supportive of our evolutionary account (Hansen & Hansen, 1988;Öhman & Dimberg, 1978), developmental experience is crucial for the normal expression of evolved traits (Bates et al, 1998;Frankenhuis & Panchanathan, 2011;Johnson, 2011;Tooby, Cosmides, & Barrett, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 52%
“…Although we point to the classic literature on preparedness for detecting angry faces as supportive of our evolutionary account (Hansen & Hansen, 1988;Öhman & Dimberg, 1978), developmental experience is crucial for the normal expression of evolved traits (Bates et al, 1998;Frankenhuis & Panchanathan, 2011;Johnson, 2011;Tooby, Cosmides, & Barrett, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 52%
“…Specifically, if young infants typically had positive everyday interactions, then their evaluative neutral point, rather than being equidistant from positive and negative evaluations, would shift closer to positive evaluations; this would 7 Some researchers (e.g., Bohner, Bless, Schwarz, & Strack, 1988;Kellermann, 1984) have attempted to rule this out by manipulating the probability and the negativity of events independently and revealing a negativity bias even when the negative and positive events occurred equally frequently (see also Eastwood, Smilek, & Merikle, 2001;Fox et al, 2000;Hansen & Hansen, 1988;Öhman, Lundqvist, & Esteves, 2001;Pratto & John, 1991). Note, however, that if in our daily lives, we do generally experience more positive than negative outcomes, and negative outcomes do therefore stand out, then when faced with an artificial research situation in which there is an equal or higher probability of negative outcomes, we might nevertheless display a negativity bias (Baumeister et al, 2001;Taylor, 1991).…”
Section: A the Negativity Bias In Attention To Emotionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, facial expressions are generally thought to be biologically rooted , comparable across cultures (Ekman et al, 1969), and they clearly evoke emotional responses in everyday life (Dimberg, 1982). Similar to pictures of emotional scenes (e.g., Alpers, 2008), emotional expressions have been shown to capture attention in visual search tasks (Hansen and Hansen, 1988;Öhman et al, 2001) and in the dot-probe deployment paradigm Mogg and Bradley, 2002). Within the framework of the somatic marker hypothesis it has been argued that the perception of an emotional expression involves the simulation of the emotional state within the relevant cortical circuitry of the observer (Adolphs, 2002).…”
Section: Emotional Facial Expressionsmentioning
confidence: 99%