1992
DOI: 10.2307/2786941
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Filling in the Blanks: A Theory of Cognitive Categories and the Structure of Social Affiliation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
123
0
1

Year Published

2004
2004
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 235 publications
(124 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
0
123
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The reliability of existing measures for relationships has been the subject of sharp debate over the last 30 years, starting with a series of landmark studies in which it was found that behavioral observations were surprisingly weakly related to reported interactions (8)(9)(10). There are multiple layers of cognitive filters that influence whether a subject reports a behavior (11). Existing research suggests that people are good at recalling long-term, but not short-term, social structures (12).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The reliability of existing measures for relationships has been the subject of sharp debate over the last 30 years, starting with a series of landmark studies in which it was found that behavioral observations were surprisingly weakly related to reported interactions (8)(9)(10). There are multiple layers of cognitive filters that influence whether a subject reports a behavior (11). Existing research suggests that people are good at recalling long-term, but not short-term, social structures (12).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…People seem not to be very good at seeing structural holes 'from the inside,' unless they already live in a hole-fi lled world. For example, in summarizing several studies (Freeman, 1992;Janicki and Larrick, 2005), he concluded, 'the summary conclusion from Freeman's and Janicik's experiments is that experience matters: people who live in a network that contains structural holes are more likely to recognize the holes in their next networks' (Burt, 2005: 77). …”
Section: Breaking Out Of Clustersmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Relational schemas help people process -memorize, interpret and recall -information about otherwise overly intricate interpersonal structures (Baldwin, 1992;Janicik & Larrick, 2005;Ridgeway, 2006). Like all cognitive representations, relational schemas do not yield perfectly accurate pictures of reality, and research has documented various systematic biases stemming from schema activation (Freeman, 1992;Kilduff et al, 2008;. Despite these biases, relational schemas are indispensable "information compression heuristics" helping people to cognitively map the network of interpersonal relations surrounding them and, as such, they are necessary to interact competently with others (Brashears, 2013;Crockett, 1982b).…”
Section: Theory People Hold a Linear-ordering Schema To Process Leadementioning
confidence: 99%
“…To test it, we performed a "network learning experiment" analogous to the one developed by Janicik and Larrick (2005: 351-2 -Study 1), who in their turn followed a long and wellestablished tradition of experimental research on relational schemas (De Soto, 1960;Henley et al, 1969;Freeman, 1992). This experimental approach leverages the fact that, although people's schemas cannot not be directly observed, they can be inferred by observing how subjects' learning rates change in schema-consistent versus schema-inconsistent conditions.…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 99%