1993
DOI: 10.1109/34.232076
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Figure-ground discrimination: a combinatorial optimization approach

Abstract: Abstract-h this paper, we attack the figure-ground discrimination problem from a combinatorial optimization perspective. In general, the solutions proposed in the past solved this problem only partially: Either the mathematical model encoding the figure-ground problem was too simple, the optimization methods that were used were not efficient enough, or they could not guarantee that the global minimum of the cost function describing the figure-ground model would be found. The method that we devised and is descr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
84
0
2

Year Published

1996
1996
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 130 publications
(87 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
(7 reference statements)
1
84
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…We note that, in contrast to other existing methods for grouping that search over the exponentially large space of all possible image curves (e.g., Herault and Horaud, 1993;Jacobs, 1993;Parent and Zucker, 1989;Williams, 1994), the Saliency Network recovers the most salient curve in time complexity that is polynomial in the size of the image. However, the network must take a single choice at every junction, and curves lying near a salient curve tend to merge into the salient curve because they can benefit from its saliency.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 94%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We note that, in contrast to other existing methods for grouping that search over the exponentially large space of all possible image curves (e.g., Herault and Horaud, 1993;Jacobs, 1993;Parent and Zucker, 1989;Williams, 1994), the Saliency Network recovers the most salient curve in time complexity that is polynomial in the size of the image. However, the network must take a single choice at every junction, and curves lying near a salient curve tend to merge into the salient curve because they can benefit from its saliency.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Other tasks in perceptual grouping are image segmentation and gap completion. For instance (Herault and Horaud, 1993;Jacobs, 1993;Martelli, 1976;Nevatia, 1988, 1992;Montanari, 1971;Parent and Zucker, 1989;Pavlidis and Liow, 1990;Weiss, 1988;Williams and Hanson, 1994) extract contours from the image according to certain optimization criteria, (Ullman, 1976;Ruthowski, 1979;Brady et al, 1980;Horn, 1983;Bruckstein and Netravali, 1990) compute optimal curves for filling in gaps, and (Brady and Grimson, 1981;Webb and Pervin, 1984;Finkel and Sajda, 1992;Grossberg and Mingolla, 1987;Heitger and von der Heydt, 1993;Mumford, 1994;Williams and Jacobs, 1995) identify occluded and subjective contours.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is work on recognizing objects from range data, considering occlusions [36], but here we are attempting to recover from them. The key to reconstruction is the knowledge that the shape of the unobserved surface is usually the same as the observed portion of a surface [23,34]. This allows us to project surfaces into occluded areas.…”
Section: Standard Shapes Allows Recovery Of Unobservable Shape and Tementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Of the work in perceptual organization with extended primitives, such as lines or arcs, the effort has been mostly to form simple, small groups of primitives such as parallels [5], convex outlines [6], ellipses [7], [28], and rectangles [8], [28], [29]. Among the frameworks that can form large groups is the solution suggested by Herault and Horaud [30]. They use simulated annealing to solve the figure ground problem.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%