2002
DOI: 10.1007/s00442-001-0858-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Fight or flight? Antipredator behavior and the escalation of coyote encounters with deer

Abstract: It is well known that prey of different size and morphology often use different antipredator strategies. The prevailing notion is that this occurs because size, morphology and weaponry determine the relative effectiveness of alternative strategies, and nowhere is this assumption more entrenched than in our view of the basic decision to stay, fight or flee. Here, we use observations of coyote (Canis latrans) packs hunting deer in winter to show that two ungulates of similar size and morphology, white-tailed dee… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

5
98
0
1

Year Published

2004
2004
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 107 publications
(111 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
5
98
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…A similar dependence of defensive response on motor capacities and habitat structure was previously described in deer species [30]. White-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus), which inhabit forests and are fast runners, tend to flee when encountering coyotes (Canis latrans), whereas mule deer (Odocoileus hemionus), which live in relatively open spaces and are moderate runners, tend to freeze or flee.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 55%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A similar dependence of defensive response on motor capacities and habitat structure was previously described in deer species [30]. White-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus), which inhabit forests and are fast runners, tend to flee when encountering coyotes (Canis latrans), whereas mule deer (Odocoileus hemionus), which live in relatively open spaces and are moderate runners, tend to freeze or flee.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 55%
“…Defensive fighting occurs only when the prey cannot avoid the predator [30]. Freezing and fleeing were previously described in a variety of prey species, from hermit crabs [36] to deer [40].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When attack is imminent, secondary defences are deployed to delay, inhibit or escape from an approaching predator. The most common secondary defence is to flee (Humphries & Driver 1970;Eibl-Eibesfeldt 1975); however, direct interactions with predators are often unavoidable (Lingle & Pellis 2002;Edut & Eilam 2004). As a result, prey may attempt to startle, threaten or confuse a predator with defensive postures and erratic, unpredictable escape sequences known as protean behaviour (Humphries & Driver 1970;Edmunds 1974;Driver & Humphries 1988).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…the predator is not hungry) (Stankowitch & Coss 2006), deimatic and protean behaviours may also be effective in assessing risk and testing predator motivation (Edmunds 1974). The decision of which defence tactic to use presumably depends on the type of predator, the severity of the threat and the environmental factors surrounding the encounter (Lima 1992; Lingle & Pellis 2002;Hoverman & Relyea 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They are highly susceptible to being captured when encountered by coyotes (Canis latrans) early in life (Lingle et al 2005); during this period, they spend the majority of their time in hiding. When a white-tailed deer fawn reaches approximately 4-5 months of age, it can gallop fast enough to outdistance coyotes in most situations and its susceptibility to being captured declines steeply (Lingle and Pellis 2002).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%