2013
DOI: 10.1167/13.3.12
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Feature specificity in attentional capture by size and color

Abstract: Top-down guidance of visual attention has classically been thought to operate in a feature-specific manner. However, recent studies have shown that top-down visual attention can also be guided by information about target-nontarget feature relations (e.g., larger, redder, brighter). Here we recommend a minimal set of cues for differentiating between relational and feature-specific attentional guidance and examine contrasting predictions for the guidance of attention by size and color stimuli in a spatial cueing… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
64
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
5

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
7
64
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This conclusion converges with Harris, Remington, and Becker's (2013) finding that in search for size, observers adopt a relational search strategy when both feature and relational strategies are available. In a modified version of Posner's (1980) attention cueing paradigm, observers indicated the orientation of a medium target line among small, or large distractors, respectively.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…This conclusion converges with Harris, Remington, and Becker's (2013) finding that in search for size, observers adopt a relational search strategy when both feature and relational strategies are available. In a modified version of Posner's (1980) attention cueing paradigm, observers indicated the orientation of a medium target line among small, or large distractors, respectively.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…The large positive cueing effect observed here for target matching cues has been reported in behavioral studies numerous times (e.g., Folk & Remington 1998;Folk et al 1992Folk et al , 1994Harris et al 2013;Lamy et al 2004;etc. ) and is generally interpreted as indicating the capture of spatial attention by cues possessing a goalrelevant property.…”
Section: Experiments 1 -Oscillatory Correlates Of Goal Directed Attentsupporting
confidence: 79%
“…Consistent with this explanation, false alarm rates were higher in the color-onset condition than in the onset-color condition, suggesting that the similarity between cue context and target was higher. In line with the context-selection hypothesis, other studies also reported cueing costs when contextual cues and targets had the same color (Harris, Remington, & Becker, 2013;Lien, Ruthruff, & Cornett, 2010;Schönhammer, Grubert, Kerzel, & Becker, 2016). Second, the disengagement hypothesis can account for the cueing costs in the color-onset condition by assuming that the color cues caused attentional capture, rapid disengagement, and spatially specific inhibition at the time the target appeared.…”
Section: Cueing Costs In the Color-onset Conditionmentioning
confidence: 71%