2016
DOI: 10.1101/085381
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Feature-coding transitions to conjunction-coding with progression through human visual cortex

Abstract: Identifying an object and distinguishing it from similar items depends upon the ability to perceive its component parts as conjoined into a cohesive whole, but the brain mechanisms underlying this ability remain elusive. The ventral visual processing pathway in primates is organized hierarchically: Neuronal responses in its early stages are sensitive to the manipulation of simple visual features whereas neuronal responses in subsequent stages are tuned to increasingly complex stimulus attributes. It is widely … Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This is important, because LOC and PRC are the two regions among those we tested that are most clearly implicated in holistic visual object-processing as measured with perceptual tasks (e.g., Malach et al 1995; Grill-Spector et al 1998; Barense et al 2007). Third, the more general finding that, for Fribbles, conjunction memory signals were most strongly correlated with conjunction memory performance in anterior sites also accords with previous findings that complex conjunctive representations are found in anterior sites during perceptual tasks (e.g., Desimone et al 1984; Kobatake and Tanaka 1994; Malach et al 1995; Kanwisher et al 1997; Kriegeskorte et al 2008; Ostwald et al 2008; Drucker and Aguirre 2009; Cowell et al 2017). Fourth, for Scenes, evidence for a conjunction memory correlation was again negligible in visual cortex and steadily increased with anterior progression, reaching a maximum not in LOC or PRC, this time, but in HC.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This is important, because LOC and PRC are the two regions among those we tested that are most clearly implicated in holistic visual object-processing as measured with perceptual tasks (e.g., Malach et al 1995; Grill-Spector et al 1998; Barense et al 2007). Third, the more general finding that, for Fribbles, conjunction memory signals were most strongly correlated with conjunction memory performance in anterior sites also accords with previous findings that complex conjunctive representations are found in anterior sites during perceptual tasks (e.g., Desimone et al 1984; Kobatake and Tanaka 1994; Malach et al 1995; Kanwisher et al 1997; Kriegeskorte et al 2008; Ostwald et al 2008; Drucker and Aguirre 2009; Cowell et al 2017). Fourth, for Scenes, evidence for a conjunction memory correlation was again negligible in visual cortex and steadily increased with anterior progression, reaching a maximum not in LOC or PRC, this time, but in HC.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…This account proposes that the role of each brain region within the ventral visual-medial temporal lobe (MTL) pathway is best explained not by which cognitive function it performs, but by the type of information it represents. Building on well-established evidence for a hierarchy of increasingly complex stimulus representations in visual cortex (Hubel and Wiesel 1965; Felleman and Van Essen 1991; Kobatake and Tanaka 1994; Malach et al 1995; Kanwisher et al 1997; Kamitani and Tong 2005; Ostwald et al 2008; Henriksson et al 2008; Kriegeskorte et al 2008; Brouwer and Heeger 2009; Serences, Saproo, et al 2009; Drucker and Aguirre 2009; Cowell et al 2017), this account suggests that the representational hierarchy extends beyond visual cortex into the MTL. Thus, the pathway houses a graded continuum of representations, beginning with simple visual features such as oriented lines in V1 and culminating in complex multi-modal conjunctions corresponding to episodes or events in HC.…”
mentioning
confidence: 79%
“…For instance, a triangle consists of the conjunction of three oriented lines intersecting at specific locations; if the lines were arranged differently, the triangle would cease to be. This definition of complexity is mirrored by the hierarchical organization of the visual processing pathway, with neural responses being tuned to simple visual features in early stages, and to increasingly complex (i.e., conjunctive) objects in later cortical stages (Cowell, Leger, & Serences, 2017). Visual processing in areas of the medial-temporal lobe (MTL) most directly connected to the hippocampus, such as perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices, is dominated by high-level objects and scenes, respectively (Martin, Douglas, Newsome, Man, & Barense, 2018;Murray, Bussey, & Saksida, 2007;Epstein & Kanwisher, 1998), as well as their spatial, temporal, and associative relations (Tsao et al, 2018;Garvert, Dolan, & Behrens, 2017;Hafting, Fyhn, Molden, Moser, & Moser, 2005).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, no such "place" code exists for olfaction, since combinations of odorants comprise the "adequate stimulus" for olfaction and a two-dimensional sensory receptor surface ca not achieve a simple low-dimensional mapping of this combination space (Mathis et al, 2016;Herz et al, 2017). Like olfaction, auditory, somatic and visual perception also involve the combinations of many basic features (Walker et al, 2011;Gomez-Ramirez et al, 2014;Cowell et al, 2017;Franke et al, 2017;Lieber and Bensmaia, 2020). Therefore, when probed in high-dimensional conditions, all PSC fields may reflect a similar mechanism of representation to that observed in chemosensory areas.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%