2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.04.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

False memories for aggressive acts

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Compared to the video data, the descriptions in the case files lack detailed information on the behavioral micro-sequences, especially because such micro-behaviors have a nonverbal and subconscious expression that rarely finds its way to self-reported accounts of the situation (Collins 2004:65-78;Laney and Takarangi 2013). Further, this issue is accentuated by the fact that police case files contain only few detailed behavioral descriptions of the bystanders/witnesses, reflecting the interest of the police in clarifying the question of guilt among alleged perpetrators and victims.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Compared to the video data, the descriptions in the case files lack detailed information on the behavioral micro-sequences, especially because such micro-behaviors have a nonverbal and subconscious expression that rarely finds its way to self-reported accounts of the situation (Collins 2004:65-78;Laney and Takarangi 2013). Further, this issue is accentuated by the fact that police case files contain only few detailed behavioral descriptions of the bystanders/witnesses, reflecting the interest of the police in clarifying the question of guilt among alleged perpetrators and victims.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, retrospective accounts of violent episodes may be inaccurate owing to cognitive constraints, false memories, self-deception, and memory failure-phenomena found to increase in times of high stress or aggression (Laney & Takarangi, 2013;Saunders, 1991;Vrij, Hope, & Fisher, 2014). Adding to this issue, because violence in the NTE often serves as a symbolic means of status and masculinity validation, patrons may provide dramatized narratives and distorted self-presentational accounts of actual events (Collins, 2008;Hochstetler et al, 2014;Jackson-Jacobs, 2004).…”
Section: Self-reports: Surveys and Interviewsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If consistent differences could be found between true and false memories, this would be a boon to forensic interviewing and the legal system generally. So far, researchers have looked for differences in confidence, detail, brain activity, consequentiality, longevity, language use, and emotional content (Bernstein, Laney, Morris, & Loftus, 2005;Lampinen, Meier, Arnal, & Leding, 2005;Laney, Bowman Fowler, Nelson, Bernstein, & Loftus, 2008;Laney & Takarangi, 2013;Sederberg et al, 2007;Vrij, 2005). So far, none of these analyses have produced useful (consistent, substantial) differences between true and false memories.…”
Section: False Memorymentioning
confidence: 96%
“…In these false feedback studies the typical sample size is over 200, and approximately a third of experimental subjects form false beliefs or memories (with this proportion exceeding half for some items; Laney & Takarangi, 2013). These numbers allow researchers to compare the false memories of some subjects to the true (that is, consistently held from before any manipulation) memories of other subjects.…”
Section: False Memorymentioning
confidence: 98%