2017
DOI: 10.7287/peerj.preprints.3486
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Fallibility in science: Responding to errors in the work of oneself and others

Abstract: Fallibility in science cuts both ways: it poses dilemmas for the scientist who discovers errors in their own work, and for those who discover errors in the work of others. The ethical response to finding errors in one's own work is clear: they should be claimed and corrected as rapidly as possible. Yet people are often reluctant to 'do the right thing' because of a perception this could lead to reputational damage. I argue that the best defence against such outcomes is adoption of open science practices, which… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 2 publications
0
6
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, it is quite possible that fears about negative reputational consequences are exaggerated. It is unclear whether and to what extent self-retractions actually damage researchers' reputations (Bishop, 2018). Recent acts of self-correction such as those by Carney (2016), which inspired our efforts in this project, Silberzahn and Uhlmann (Silberzahn et al, 2014), Inzlicht (2016), Willén (2018), and Gervais (2017) have received positive reactions from within the psychological community.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Finally, it is quite possible that fears about negative reputational consequences are exaggerated. It is unclear whether and to what extent self-retractions actually damage researchers' reputations (Bishop, 2018). Recent acts of self-correction such as those by Carney (2016), which inspired our efforts in this project, Silberzahn and Uhlmann (Silberzahn et al, 2014), Inzlicht (2016), Willén (2018), and Gervais (2017) have received positive reactions from within the psychological community.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Dans les publications scientifiques, les erreurs statistiques sont plus communes que ce que l’on peut imaginer de prime abord, et ce, dans différentes disciplines, que ce soit en psychologie ( Bakker & Wicherts, 2011 ; Nuijten, Hartgerink, van Assen, Epskamp, & Wicherts, 2016 ; Rouder, Haaf, & Snyder, 2019 ), en médecine ( García-Berthou & Alcaraz, 2004 ; Murphy, 2004 ), en psychiatrie ( Berle & Starcevic, 2007 ), en management ( Bergh, Sharp, Aguinis, & Li, 2017 ) ou en philosophie expérimentale ( Colombo, Duev, Nuijten, & Sprenger, 2018 ). Les erreurs étant humaines (et source d’embarras, Bishop, 2018 ), il serait extraordinairement étonnant de n’en observer aucune. Ces erreurs peuvent être de nature très diverse et varier en gravité.…”
Section: L’adoption Des Pratiques De Recherche Ouvertes Comme Piste Dunclassified
“…Errors occur in public and in private, and while "making code and data open does not prevent errors, […] it does make it possible to detect them. […] People often worry that if they make their code and data open, errors will be found, but that is really the whole point: We need to make code and data open because this is how the errors can be found" [ 108 ]. The individuals who document the research don’t have to be the same people who conduct the research: assigning different people to document versus run a study encourages generally understandable documentation.…”
Section: Rule 6: Document Everythingmentioning
confidence: 99%