2016
DOI: 10.1002/ejsp.2207
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Faking revisited: Exerting strategic control over performance on the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure

Abstract: . Electronic mail should be sent to sean.hughes@ugent.be.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
15
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
(57 reference statements)
0
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, due to its block-based nature, the IRAP is limited to assessing implicit beliefs toward a single set of beliefs at a time (i.e., for a given pair of blocks with their associated reference beliefs). In addition, IRAP scores have been shown to be susceptible to faking attempts (Hughes et al, 2016) and often exhibit moderate reliability only (e.g., Remue et al, 2013, 2014; see also Gawronski and De Houwer, 2014). Finally, the IRAP is also held back by substantial dropout rates in participants (more than 20% dropout is reported among university students in Remue et al, 2013; for a discussion, see De Houwer et al, 2015) – an issue that is thought to be attributable to the trial-by-trial response key reassignment.…”
Section: Issue 3: Focus On Associations Versus Beliefsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, due to its block-based nature, the IRAP is limited to assessing implicit beliefs toward a single set of beliefs at a time (i.e., for a given pair of blocks with their associated reference beliefs). In addition, IRAP scores have been shown to be susceptible to faking attempts (Hughes et al, 2016) and often exhibit moderate reliability only (e.g., Remue et al, 2013, 2014; see also Gawronski and De Houwer, 2014). Finally, the IRAP is also held back by substantial dropout rates in participants (more than 20% dropout is reported among university students in Remue et al, 2013; for a discussion, see De Houwer et al, 2015) – an issue that is thought to be attributable to the trial-by-trial response key reassignment.…”
Section: Issue 3: Focus On Associations Versus Beliefsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are two important points to highlight here. First, it appears difficult to fake responding on the IRAP without a concrete strategy and/or detailed instructions which participants are unlikely to spontaneously devise (Drake et al 2016;Hughes et al 2016;McKenna et al 2007). Also, if the Time 2 results represented more effortful, conscious responding, it is interesting to note that there was still an explicit Male-STEM/Female-Arts bias among the groups.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The should-IRAP would be identical to the are-IRAP, with the exception that the statement at the top of the screen would instead be "Men should be" or "Women should be". In terms of its implicitness, the IRAP has been argued to capture responding under fast and unintentional conditions (Barnes-Holmes et al, 2010), although some evidence disputes its unintentional nature (Drake et al, 2016;Hughes et al, 2016).…”
Section: Overview Of Relational Implicit Measuresmentioning
confidence: 99%