2011
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023223
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Fairness Expectations and Altruistic Sharing in 15-Month-Old Human Infants

Abstract: Human cooperation is a key driving force behind the evolutionary success of our hominin lineage. At the proximate level, biologists and social scientists have identified other-regarding preferences – such as fairness based on egalitarian motives, and altruism – as likely candidates for fostering large-scale cooperation. A critical question concerns the ontogenetic origins of these constituents of cooperative behavior, as well as whether they emerge independently or in an interrelated fashion. The answer to thi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

17
321
2
20

Year Published

2015
2015
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 399 publications
(360 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
17
321
2
20
Order By: Relevance
“…In this game, children were given two toys to play with (11). One toy was a very desirable ring, which makes sounds while shaking it and has colorful paintings on the other side.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In this game, children were given two toys to play with (11). One toy was a very desirable ring, which makes sounds while shaking it and has colorful paintings on the other side.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is some evidence that an improvement in one prosocial ability may relate to increased abilities in other prosocial areas. For example, 15-mo-old infants who chose to share a toy they preferred (compared with a nonpreferred toy or no toy at all) with an experimenter also attended significantly longer to a third-party interaction in which the allocation of resources among conspecifics was unequal (11). However, another body of literature suggests that early prosocial abilities are not necessarily related (12).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These findings suggested that children were more strongly guided by the recipient"s gender and race category, rather than a desire to be fair. However, resource distribution has been suggested as an important developing ground for children"s sense of fairness (Fehr, Bernhard, & Rockenbach, 2008) and as such, these findings contrast with literature which suggest a sense of fairness and equality forms from early infancy (Geraci & Surian, 2011;Schmidt & Sommerville, 2011;Sloane, Baillargeon, & Premack, 2012), leading children to favour equal resource distribution (e.g., Cooley & Killen, 2015). According to Fehr and colleagues (2008), most 7-to 8-year-old children are adverse to either advantageous or disadvantageous resource distribution.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Thus, a group norm of cooperation is often salient during resource allocation reflecting the generic societal moral norm (Hamann et al, 2011;Schmidt & Sommerville, 2011). Yet peer ingroup and outgroup social norms are not always cooperative and peer groups do sometimes advocate for competition within competitive intergroup resource allocation scenarios, from seven years onwards (Dejesus, Rhodes, & Kinzler, 2014;Shaw, DeScioli, & Olson, 2012).…”
Section: Group Norms Intergroup Resource Allocation and Reasoning Ammentioning
confidence: 99%