1983
DOI: 10.3758/bf03212304
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Factors affecting schedule-induced wood-chewing in rats: Percentage and rate of reinforcement, and operant requirement

Abstract: In Experiment I, rats were allowed to acquire either schedule-induced drinking or scheduleinduced wood-chewing behavior under a fixed-interval (FI) 60-sec schedule of food reinforcement, following which food was omitted from 20% and then 50% of interreinforcement intervals. Omission of food severely disrupted induced drinking but had relatively little effect on induced wood-chewing. Experiment 2 investigated wood-chewing as a function of reinforcement rate, using a range of FI schedules from 5 to 180 sec in du… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

2
4
0

Year Published

1984
1984
1993
1993

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
2
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…From the present analy-sis of the overall and temporal interaction of licking and running, it is clear that induced behavior does not necessarily have priority over noninduced behavior (see also Rileyet al, 1981;Roper, 1978;Segal, 1969). These data support the growing evidence that induced and noninduced behaviors may have no distinguishing characteristics other than induction (see Killeen, 1975;Roper & Crossland, 1982;Roper, Edwards, & Crossland, 1983;Wetherington, 1982).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 76%
“…From the present analy-sis of the overall and temporal interaction of licking and running, it is clear that induced behavior does not necessarily have priority over noninduced behavior (see also Rileyet al, 1981;Roper, 1978;Segal, 1969). These data support the growing evidence that induced and noninduced behaviors may have no distinguishing characteristics other than induction (see Killeen, 1975;Roper & Crossland, 1982;Roper, Edwards, & Crossland, 1983;Wetherington, 1982).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 76%
“…The decline in volume drunk by the pellet rats in the omitted-food condition compared with that drunk in the scheduled condition is commensurate with the decreased amount of food received in the former condition, suggesting that the pelletfed rats were drinking very little in the no-food intervals. This result agrees with observations on time spent licking the water spout in an omitted-food condition by rats receiving pellets (Roper et al, 1983). The interval data on spout licking from the present study will be presented in a later section.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 91%
“…In the omitted-food condition, no food was delivered on selected intervals of the schedule of reinforcement. The water licking of rats showing pellet polydipsia is similar in no-food intervals of omitted-food conditions to that in no-food conditions (Roper, 1981;Roper, Edwards, & Crossland, 1983). If rats receiving powder truly are not polydipsic, then their drinking under schedule should be the same as their drinking in the massed-food condition and in the no-food intervals of the omitted-food condition.…”
Section: Excessiveness Of Drinkingmentioning
confidence: 80%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The temporal distributions of food-related behavior (head-in-feeder) quickly stabilized in Phase 2, when no access was allowed to the running wheel, oak chewing block, or the drinking tube. Roper, Edwards, and Crossland (1983) proposed that schedule-induced chewing may develop only if rats have ample exposure to the periodic food schedule before access to the oak block is allowed. The purpose of Phase 2 was simply to allow this exposure in order to encourage as many activities as possible to become induced.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%