2002
DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x02430087
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Facial expression of pain, empathy, evolution, and social learning

Abstract: The experience of pain appears to be associated, from early infancy and across pain stimuli, with a consistent facial expression in humans. A social function is proposed for this: the communication of pain and the need for help to observers, to whom information about danger is of value, and who may provide help within a kin or cooperative relationship. Some commentators have asserted that the evidence is insufficient to account for the consistency of the face, as judged by technical means or in the perceptions… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
26
0
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 222 publications
2
26
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, when observing injury or pain in another, perceptions of alarm and danger may be activated to promote escape or other protective responses (Yamada and Decety 2009;Eccleston and Crombez 1999;Williams 2002;Decety 2011;Ibáñez et al 2011). The ''threat value of pain hypothesis'' (TVPH) posits observing others' pain is potentially threatening to observers, and activates an early, threat-detection system instead of evoking automatic empathic responses (Ibáñez et al 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, when observing injury or pain in another, perceptions of alarm and danger may be activated to promote escape or other protective responses (Yamada and Decety 2009;Eccleston and Crombez 1999;Williams 2002;Decety 2011;Ibáñez et al 2011). The ''threat value of pain hypothesis'' (TVPH) posits observing others' pain is potentially threatening to observers, and activates an early, threat-detection system instead of evoking automatic empathic responses (Ibáñez et al 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nonverbal expressions of pain by a sufferer are known to reflect the pain experience (Craig 2009;Craig et al 2001;Hadjistavropoulos and Craig 2002;Prkachin and Craig 1995;Williams 2002). Valid nonverbal expressions of pain are identified by examining how closely various cues are related to sufferers' self-reported pain ratings.…”
Section: Patient-centered Provider Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is, however, some evidence that cues of pain can be altered, especially by social context and social interactions (for reviews see Williams 2002;Krahé et al 2013). For example, Peeters and Vlaeyen (2011) demonstrated that participants receiving the same number of electric shocks showed fewer facial expressions of pain (coded with the Child Facial Coding System; Chambers et al 1996) when they were falsely told that a confederate inflicted the most electric shocks on them compared to participants who were led to believe that they were given the fewest number of shocks.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some of these phenomena are grounded on the theory of mind (ToM) [Whiten, 1991], which studies the human ability to attribute mental states (like beliefs, desires, or intentions) to other people, and to use those mental states to reason or to feel like others would do. Besides, the capacity of individuals to identify emotional signals in facial expressions or body gestures allows these phenomena to take place (see e.g., [Hess et al, 1998;Williams, …”
Section: Emotions and Social Interactionmentioning
confidence: 99%