2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.10.009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Extremely long-term memory and familiarity after 12 years

Abstract: In 2006 Mitchell demonstrated that implicit memory was robust to decay. He showed that the ability to identify fragments of pictures seen 17 years before was significantly higher than for new stimuli. Is this true only for implicit memory? In this study, we tested whether explicit memory was still possible for drawings (n = 144) that had been presented once or three times, two seconds each time on average, approximately 12 years earlier. Surprisingly, our data reveal that our participants were able to recogniz… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One key item of information retrieved during this process is whether or not objects are familiar. The system backing this ability has a massive and detailed storage capacity (Brady et al 2008), can support very long-term memory (even for objects initially seen for only a few seconds (Larzabal et al 2018)), and is also quite rapid as behavioral responses start to occur around 350-400 ms following presentation of the stimulus (Besson et al 2012). This shows that the ability to recognize familiar objects is a powerful natural cognitive system.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One key item of information retrieved during this process is whether or not objects are familiar. The system backing this ability has a massive and detailed storage capacity (Brady et al 2008), can support very long-term memory (even for objects initially seen for only a few seconds (Larzabal et al 2018)), and is also quite rapid as behavioral responses start to occur around 350-400 ms following presentation of the stimulus (Besson et al 2012). This shows that the ability to recognize familiar objects is a powerful natural cognitive system.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Based on such findings, it has been assumed that there is a third subtype of memory beyond the awareness-dependent (explicit) subtypes of recollection and familiarity: an unconscious (implicit) subtype of memory, which is not accompanied by a phenomenal (i.e., conscious) experience of remembering, termed "recognition without awareness" or "implicit recognition" (for a review, see Voss et al, 2012). Such an implicit subtype of memory is also well supported by studies on the so-called repetition priming effect, demonstrating that a brief exposure to a visual picture without mentioning that memory will be tested later leads to a processing benefit when the picture is encountered again after several days, months, or even years, with participants often being unaware of their memory abilities (Mitchell and Brown, 1988;Mitchell, 2006;Larzabal et al, 2018;Mitchell et al, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Specifically, a familiar identification requires an eyewitness to additionally determine who the person is and where they know her from—the crime or some other context (see Smith, Volna, & Ewing, 2016). As a result, familiarity judgments and identifications are impacted by different factors and not synonymous—that is, an accurate familiarity judgment does not guarantee an accurate identification because it may not be correctly associated with the crime and perpetrator (see Kersten & Earles, 2017; Larzabal, Tramoni, Muratot, Thorpe, & Barbeau, 2018).…”
Section: Part 1: What Are Familiar Identifications and How Accurate A...mentioning
confidence: 99%