2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.009
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Exploring mathematics problems prepares children to learn from instruction

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
90
4
3

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 123 publications
(109 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
5
90
4
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Learners sometimes struggle to provide relevant explanations, and, in these cases, self-explanation often does not improve learning (e.g., Broers & Imbos, 2005;DeCaro & Rittle-Johnson, 2012;Matthews & Rittle-Johnson, 2009;Mwangi & Sweller, 1998). In other cases, learners' explanations focus their attention on particular types of information, decreasing their attention to, and learning of, other information (e.g., Berthold et al, 2011;Kuhn & Katz, 2009;.…”
Section: Constraints On When Prompting For Explanation Aids Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Learners sometimes struggle to provide relevant explanations, and, in these cases, self-explanation often does not improve learning (e.g., Broers & Imbos, 2005;DeCaro & Rittle-Johnson, 2012;Matthews & Rittle-Johnson, 2009;Mwangi & Sweller, 1998). In other cases, learners' explanations focus their attention on particular types of information, decreasing their attention to, and learning of, other information (e.g., Berthold et al, 2011;Kuhn & Katz, 2009;.…”
Section: Constraints On When Prompting For Explanation Aids Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…New technologies can benefit students' learning, but the details of implementing such technologies are crucial to their success, for instance in terms of the social engineering of the learning environment, sequencing of instruction/exploration, activity design, as well as the level of allowed discovery/inquiry. DeCaro and Rittle-Johnson (2012) have shown that when open-ended activities precede formal instruction (such as exploring quantities before receiving explanation about math equivalence), students are more able to solve math problems and achieve deeper conceptual knowledge. This approach was also useful to adults learning college-level content, as is pointed out by Schneider et al (2013).…”
Section: Stem Instructions Creativity and Technologymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The mathematical equivalence assessment was adapted from previous research (DeCaro & Rittle-Johnson, 2012;Matthews, Rittle-Johnson, McEldoon, & Taylor, 2012;Rittle-Johnson, Taylor, Matthews, & McEldoon, 2011). The assessment included four subscales (see Table 2): Conceptual Knowledge items evaluated both individuals' understanding of the meaning of the equal sign as a relational symbol and the structure of equations.…”
Section: Mathematical Equivalence Assessmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Two additional Conceptual Knowledge items were included, based on previous research (e.g., DeCaro & Rittle-Johnson, 2012;RittleJohnson & Alibali, 1999). For these two items, students were shown a mathematical equivalence problem for 5 seconds and were asked to write down the problem from memory.…”
Section: Mathematical Equivalence Assessmentmentioning
confidence: 99%