2010
DOI: 10.1167/8.6.337
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Exploring aesthetic principles of spatial composition through stock photography

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We discovered these compositional biases by studying images of meaningful objects that we created for this purpose, but evidence for the horizontal biases are evident in suitably chosen corpora of paintings, drawings, or photographic images. Gardner et al (2008) found evidence of the horizontal inward bias for single-object images in stock photography databases, and Bertamini et al (2011) did so in paintings and drawings of animals in medieval bestiaries. We expect that similar corroboration of the present vertical compositional effects could be found in appropriate samples of photographs, paintings, and/or drawings, and we plan to do so in the future.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…We discovered these compositional biases by studying images of meaningful objects that we created for this purpose, but evidence for the horizontal biases are evident in suitably chosen corpora of paintings, drawings, or photographic images. Gardner et al (2008) found evidence of the horizontal inward bias for single-object images in stock photography databases, and Bertamini et al (2011) did so in paintings and drawings of animals in medieval bestiaries. We expect that similar corroboration of the present vertical compositional effects could be found in appropriate samples of photographs, paintings, and/or drawings, and we plan to do so in the future.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…In terms of the factors mentioned above, the "best" picture of a given object should be a high-contrast image of the object at (or near) the center of the frame portrayed in its most canonical perspective and at its canonical size. Such a picture might indeed be the best image if the intent of the photographer was simply to portray that object most clearly, as is often the goal in stock photography [see Gardner et al (2008) and databases of stock photography, such as http://www.corbis.com or http://www.gettyimages.com], and in naturalists' drawings of plants and animals [eg Audubon's images: see Bertamini et al (2011)]. In such cases there should indeed be consistency between aesthetic judgments and recognition performance, as fluency theory predicts.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite the high prevalence of photography in the everyday world, empirical studies of the psychological basis of photography are, like philosophical analyses of photography (Dobel et al 2007;Maynard 2001), quite rare. Important exceptions are the study of Axelsson (2007), who contrasted the responses of professional photographers and non-expert photographers to photographic images; that of Palmer et al (2008), who asked participants to take digital photographs of single objects within a constrained environment; and the study of Gardner et al (2008), which looked at tendencies of focal objects in stock photographs to be near the centre or oriented toward the centre. As an experimental technique for manipulating images, cropping has been included in the taxonomy of Tinio and Leder (2009) as a`composition-level manipulation', between global or`surface-level manipulations' (eg contrast, focus), and local or`semantic-level manipulations' (eg altering image content by rearranging elements within an image), cropping altering both local and global aspects of an image so that it alters``complexity, symmetry and balance'' (page 55).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%