1988
DOI: 10.21236/ada201093
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Explanatory Coherence and Belief Revision in Naive Physics

Abstract: MEUR~RY CLASSIF'C~rIoN OF rw.S RIAGZ REPORT DOCUMENTATION PAG I&. REPORT SECURITY CLASSIFICATION 1b. RESTRICTIVE MARKINGS Unclassified 24. SECUJRI.TY CLASSIAI4TION AUTHIORITY 3. 0ISTRIeufloN/AVAILABIurY OF REPORT Approved for public release; Zt. 0ECLASSiFICArIONIOOWNGRA0ImG SCHEDULEdstiuonniiite 4. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION REPORT NUMBER(S) s. MONIrORING ORGANIzArioN REPORT NUMBER(S) UP ITT/LROC/ONR/AP S-li Ba.. 4AME OF PERFORMING ORGANIZATION 6b. OFFICE SYMBOL 7a. NAME QF MONITORING ORGANIZATiON Learning Resear… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
59
0

Year Published

1991
1991
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(59 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
0
59
0
Order By: Relevance
“…amples that are superficially similar to the problem but that do not share the same solution or solution structure (Reed, Ackinc1ose, & Voss, 1990;Ross, 1987Ross, , 1989. For example, Ranney and Thagard (1988;see also Caramazza, McCloskey, & Green, 1981) reported that some people predicted that a pendulum bob would follow a curved path, rather than falling straight down, if it was released at the top of the pendulum's arc. This answer was partially based on people's erroneous recall of children jumping from swings; they seemed to forget, or not notice, that children jump before the swing reaches the top of its arc.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…amples that are superficially similar to the problem but that do not share the same solution or solution structure (Reed, Ackinc1ose, & Voss, 1990;Ross, 1987Ross, , 1989. For example, Ranney and Thagard (1988;see also Caramazza, McCloskey, & Green, 1981) reported that some people predicted that a pendulum bob would follow a curved path, rather than falling straight down, if it was released at the top of the pendulum's arc. This answer was partially based on people's erroneous recall of children jumping from swings; they seemed to forget, or not notice, that children jump before the swing reaches the top of its arc.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is still a debate in the literature as to whether impetus notions present a consistent belief system (e.g., McCloskey, 1983) or whether they are just the result of a fragmented and situational understanding of physical phenomena (e.g., diSessa, 1983(e.g., diSessa, , 1988Ranney, 1988Ranney, , 1994. For instance, Ranney and Thagard (1988) found that the same participant might give responses reflecting different motion models (impetus or Newtonian) to different problems, with no correlation of response types across problems (see also Cooke &Breedin, 1994, andRanney's, 1994, commentary). That is, people may hold impetus beliefs for McCloskey's curved tube problem, while at the same time giving Newtonian responses regarding horizontal projectile motion.…”
Section: Naive Physics Studiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In some studies, major belief change takes more time and shows up only over years as students acquire additional knowledge (e.g., Karmiloff-Smith, 1988; Chinn & Brewer Anomalous Data -12 Vosniadou & Brewer, 1992). In other studies, some theory change in response to anomalous data occurs within one or a few experimental sessions or within several classroom lessons (e.g., Alvermann & Hynd, 1989;Brown & Clement, 1989;Burbules & Linn, 1988;Dunbar & Schunn, 1990;Johsua & Dupin, 1987;Levin, Siegler, Druyan, & Gardosh, 1990;Ranney & Thagard, 1988;Rowell & Dawson, 1983;Zietsman & Hewson, 1986). For example, Rowell and Dawson (1983) found that 3 of 12 subjects who held a weight theory of water displacement changed to a volume theory after observing anomalous data that supported the volume theory.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%