2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.pragma.2016.07.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Explaining interactive alignment: A multimodal and multifactorial account

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
39
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
(47 reference statements)
0
39
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus in this case adjacent speech turns are taken as the unit of analysis 5 , where the aligned lexical item or gesture can occur in any position within those turns (e.g., Fusaroli et al, 2017, for lexical alignment in free interaction and Map Task interactions). It is also possible to look at adjacent behavior independent of speech turns, e.g., by comparing a gesture that depicts a particular object with the next gesture that is produced (by the other speaker) to depict that same object in a spot-the-differences game (Oben & Brône, 2016). Hence behaviors are "grouped" based on their sequential relationin this case, adjacency.…”
Section: Sequencementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus in this case adjacent speech turns are taken as the unit of analysis 5 , where the aligned lexical item or gesture can occur in any position within those turns (e.g., Fusaroli et al, 2017, for lexical alignment in free interaction and Map Task interactions). It is also possible to look at adjacent behavior independent of speech turns, e.g., by comparing a gesture that depicts a particular object with the next gesture that is produced (by the other speaker) to depict that same object in a spot-the-differences game (Oben & Brône, 2016). Hence behaviors are "grouped" based on their sequential relationin this case, adjacency.…”
Section: Sequencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The term (interactive) alignment was originally introduced by Pickering and Garrod (2004) to refer to the interpersonal alignment of mental representations underlying linguistic behavior. However, various scholars have used the same term to simply refer to observable similarities in communicative behavior itself (e.g., Bergmann & Kopp, 2012;Fusaroli et al, 2017;Howes et al, 2010;Oben & Brône, 2016). Of course, the two senses are related (since inferences about mental representations are often made on the basis of observed behavior), but in light of theoretical discussions it is important to keep them apart.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some variation across interlocutors may be due to differences in amount of exposure to their speech. More exposure to a construction or a lexical item increases the probability that a subject will subsequently produce that item (Kaschak et al, 2011;Oben & Brône, 2016), though cumulative effects in phonetic convergence are less clear (e.g. Babel, 2012;Gijssels et al, 2016).…”
Section: Social Mediationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…DID is sometimes used to quantify convergence in conversational tasks, looking at the change from the two speakers' starting distance and ending distance (e.g., Pardo, Cajori Jay, & Krauss, 2010), or their distance earlier and later in the conversation (e.g., Levitan & Hirschberg, 2011;Abel & Babel, 2017). When comparing interlocutors' productions within a shared conversation, conversations may appear to be convergent due to participants independently being similarly influenced by the task, e.g., speaking more quickly as they become familiar with the task or their interlocutor; some studies compare distance between interlocutors to distance between individuals who were not interacting with each other, to control for such effects (e.g., Sanker, 2015;Oben & Brône, 2016), though many do not. In AXB or similar perceptual testing of distance, the X reference point for the interlocutor is sometimes taken from the middle of the conversation, compared to the other speaker's productions before and after the conversation or before and during the conversation (e.g., Pardo, 2006).…”
Section: Modelling Convergencementioning
confidence: 99%