2009
DOI: 10.1167/9.11.15
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Experience-dependent changes in the topography of visual crowding

Abstract: The present work examined discrimination accuracy for targets that were presented either alone in the visual field (clean displays) or embedded within a dense array of letter distractors (crowded displays). The strength of visual crowding varied strongly across the four quadrants of the visual field. Furthermore, this spatial bias in crowding was strongly influenced by the observers’ prior experience with specific distractor stimuli. Observers who were monolingual readers of English experienced amplified crowd… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
8
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
1
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Because the goals of our study necessitated that we observe attention effects in both accuracy and RT, we implemented a titration procedure to ensure that the target gap was not salient enough to 1) capture attention in the absence of ongoing top-down deployment or 2) prevent us from observing effects of signal enhancement as a result of attentional deployment. The size of the small gap in the target square frame was titrated throughout each experimental session to achieve an overall performance criterion of 60% to 70% (similar criteria were used in Scolari & Awh (2019) and Williamson et al (2009)). For each participant, the gap was initially set to 0.25°, or one-quarter the size of the line segment.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because the goals of our study necessitated that we observe attention effects in both accuracy and RT, we implemented a titration procedure to ensure that the target gap was not salient enough to 1) capture attention in the absence of ongoing top-down deployment or 2) prevent us from observing effects of signal enhancement as a result of attentional deployment. The size of the small gap in the target square frame was titrated throughout each experimental session to achieve an overall performance criterion of 60% to 70% (similar criteria were used in Scolari & Awh (2019) and Williamson et al (2009)). For each participant, the gap was initially set to 0.25°, or one-quarter the size of the line segment.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This protracted development of visual processing might contribute to, or even determine advances in, reading development. Nonetheless, the reversed direction of effects is also possible given that it has been shown that reading acquisition alters the organization of early visual (letter features) processing (Dehaene, Cohen, Morais, & Kolinsky, 2015) and that reading experience induces top-down influences on visual crowding (Williamson, Scolari, Jeong, Kim, & Awh, 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, videogame players experience a smaller crowding effect than do nonplayers (Green & Bavelier, 2007 ). When numbers in a recognition experiment were flanked by Roman letters, performance decreased in the upper left visual field for native English speakers, but not for speakers of Asian languages (e.g., Japanese, Chinese or Korean; Williamson, Scolari, Jeong, Kim, & Awh, 2009 ). Crowding for upright face recognition is stronger when the flankers are upright rather than inverted faces (Louie et al, 2007 ); sensitivity to the orientation of faces is thought to be related to our perceptual experience with upright faces (Bukach, Gauthier, & Tarr, 2006 ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%