The platform will undergo maintenance on Sep 14 at about 7:45 AM EST and will be unavailable for approximately 2 hours.
2020
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-020-05756-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Executive functions in motor imagery: support for the motor-cognitive model over the functional equivalence model

Abstract: The motor-cognitive model holds that motor imagery relies on executive resources to a much greater extent than do overt actions. According to this view, engaging executive resources with an interference task during motor imagery or overt actions will lead to a greater lengthening of the time required to imagine a movement than to execute it physically. This model is in contrast to a currently popular view, the functional equivalence model, which holds that motor imagery and overt action use identical mental pr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
37
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(44 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
6
37
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Furthermore, the functional equivalence perspective predicts little difference in neural activity between motor simulation and action execution or systematic timing errors (Glover & Baran, 2017 ). Because we have found clear differences between the simulation conditions studied in the present study, our data may provide further support aligned with the growing behavioral, neurophysiological, and conceptual evidence for the motor cognitive model of motor simulation, rather than functional equivalence (Glover et al., 2020 ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…Furthermore, the functional equivalence perspective predicts little difference in neural activity between motor simulation and action execution or systematic timing errors (Glover & Baran, 2017 ). Because we have found clear differences between the simulation conditions studied in the present study, our data may provide further support aligned with the growing behavioral, neurophysiological, and conceptual evidence for the motor cognitive model of motor simulation, rather than functional equivalence (Glover et al., 2020 ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…It is possible that when playing music in one's head, there is the luxury to pause or dwell on a particular moment. Because imagery involves additional mechanisms of generation and maintenance as well executive processes (Glover et al, 2020), this could lengthen processing, without apparently increasing the cognitive workload compared to actually listening (Figure 4).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The results of experiment 4 suggested MI priming does not reflect increased motorcognitive load. At first glance this contradictsGlover & Baran's (2017;Glover et al, 2020) motor-cognitive model (MCM) of MI. The MCM was motivated by the finding that although MI performance generally adheres to biomechanical constraints of the body (de Lange et al,…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Although this is a useful heuristic for researchers investigating commonalities between MI and action proper, FE and the related neural simulation theory (Jeannerod, 2001) may be unsuitable for understanding mechanisms that are unique to MI (Glover & Baran, 2017;O'Shea & Moran, 2017). Accumulated behavioral and neural evidence demonstrates differences between MI and action that reflects this problem (Glover et al, 2020;Hanakawa et al, 2008;Ingram et al, 2019;Kranczioch et al, 2009Kranczioch et al, , 2010Lebon et al, 2019;Solomon et al, 2019). To examine the FE hypothesis in more detail, in this article we present a series of behavioral experiments which directly compare the effects of MI and MP on priming subsequent actions in an S1-S2 response priming paradigm.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%