2001
DOI: 10.1017/s0021963001006679
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Executive Functioning in High-functioning Children with Autism

Abstract: Executive functioning was investigated in 34 children (24 boys and 10 girls) with developmental language disorder (DLD) and 21 children (18 boys and 3 girls) with high-functioning autistic disorder (HAD) matched on Full Scale IQ, Nonverbal IQ, age (mean age 9 year, 1 month), and SES. The DLD group had a Verbal IQ that was 10 points higher than the HAD group. These children were given the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), the Mazes subtest from the WISC-R, the Underlining test, and the Rapid Automatized Namin… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

3
52
1
1

Year Published

2004
2004
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 58 publications
(57 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
3
52
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For IQ testing, participants 18 years of age and younger completed the Differential Ability Scales-2nd edition (DAS-II; Elliot 2007) (ASD = 37; Control = 31) or the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler 1999) (ASD = 9; Control = 9), and participants over 18 years of age completed the WASI (ASD = 14, Control = 15). While verbal IQ (VIQ) has been previously shown to be associated with executive function impairments in ASD (Bishop and Norbury 2005; Liss et al 2001; Pellicano 2007), we matched groups on NVIQ because the PCET places limited verbal demands on participants. Further, many individuals with ASD have more severe deficits in verbal compared to nonverbal IQ, suggesting that matching individuals with ASD and typically developing controls on verbal IQ may result in an ASD sample that is less representative of the broader population (Munson et al 2008).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For IQ testing, participants 18 years of age and younger completed the Differential Ability Scales-2nd edition (DAS-II; Elliot 2007) (ASD = 37; Control = 31) or the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler 1999) (ASD = 9; Control = 9), and participants over 18 years of age completed the WASI (ASD = 14, Control = 15). While verbal IQ (VIQ) has been previously shown to be associated with executive function impairments in ASD (Bishop and Norbury 2005; Liss et al 2001; Pellicano 2007), we matched groups on NVIQ because the PCET places limited verbal demands on participants. Further, many individuals with ASD have more severe deficits in verbal compared to nonverbal IQ, suggesting that matching individuals with ASD and typically developing controls on verbal IQ may result in an ASD sample that is less representative of the broader population (Munson et al 2008).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, there is also evidence that executive function deficits are not specific to autism (e.g. Baron-Cohen and Robertson, 1995;Griffith et al, 1999;Ozonoff, 1997;Pennington et al, 1997;Sergeant et al, 2002) and that they may be reduced or absent in the highest functioning individuals, especially those with Asperger syndrome (Liss et al, 2001;Rinehart et al, 2001). Thus, even though executive function deficits are present in persons with autism, they are, by themselves, probably insufficient to account for the behavioral manifestations of autism, apart from performance on ToM and some other cognitive tasks.…”
Section: Heterogeneity Of Behavioral Symptoms In Autismmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Cognitive flexibility is impaired in adults and children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) when performance is measured in the laboratory (Lopez, Lincoln, Ozonoff, & Lai, 2005; Maes, Eling, Wezenberg, Vissers, & Kan, 2010; Ozonoff et al, 2004; Schmitz et al, 2006; Solomon, Ozonoff, Cummings, & Carter, 2008; Solomon et al, 2008; South, Ozonoff, & McMahon, 2007; Yerys et al, 2009; but see Goldberg et al, 2005; Happé, Booth, Charlton, & Hughes, 2006; Liss et al, 2001; Poljac et al, 2010). Furthermore, parents' reports of cognitive flexibility in everyday settings suggest significant impairments in children with ASD (Boyd, McBee, Holtzclaw, Baranek, & Bodfish, 2009; Gioia, Isquith, Kenworthy, & Barton, 2002; Kenworthy, Black, Harrison, Della Rosa, & Wallace, 2009).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…F. Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Sergeant, Geurts, & Oosterlaan, 2002), these findings have significant limitations. Having a computer providing feedback has yielded better inductive reasoning cognitive flexibility performance in ASD relative to controls than human feedback (Ozonoff, 1995; Pascualvaca, Fantie, Papageorgiou, & Mirsky, 1998), and co-varying verbal intelligence even when ASD and controls are matched on this variable has been shown to eliminate group differences on inductive reasoning tasks (Liss et al, 2001). The simplified format of explicit tasks may explain why there is less consistency in identifying impairments for individuals with ASD (Bíró & Russell, 2001; Schmitz et al, 2006; Solomon et al, 2008, 2009).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%