2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

15
199
2

Year Published

2009
2009
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 214 publications
(219 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
15
199
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Tucker and Ellis (2004) demonstrated that showing a graspable object's name produced a congruency effect on manual responses similar to the one commonly registered in visual object categorization studies. Similar effects were reported in Bub, Masson, and Cree (2008) for both functional (grasping) and volumetric (lifting) actions (see also Lindemann et al, 2006). Interestingly, while elicitation of affordance effects from observing objects may be bound by the limits of reachable space, objects' names can act as activation cues to stimulus-response compatibility effects for objects within and outside of reachable space (Ferri et al, 2011).…”
Section: Visual and Linguistic Cues To Manual Grasp Affordancesmentioning
confidence: 59%
“…Tucker and Ellis (2004) demonstrated that showing a graspable object's name produced a congruency effect on manual responses similar to the one commonly registered in visual object categorization studies. Similar effects were reported in Bub, Masson, and Cree (2008) for both functional (grasping) and volumetric (lifting) actions (see also Lindemann et al, 2006). Interestingly, while elicitation of affordance effects from observing objects may be bound by the limits of reachable space, objects' names can act as activation cues to stimulus-response compatibility effects for objects within and outside of reachable space (Ferri et al, 2011).…”
Section: Visual and Linguistic Cues To Manual Grasp Affordancesmentioning
confidence: 59%
“…Previous studies have indicated that action information about objects is automatically activated when objects are perceived (e.g. Bub, Masson, & Cree, 2008) and that an important aspect of this knowledge consists of the object's prototypical end locations (van Elk, van Schie, & Bekkering, 2009). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These sensorimotor lexical representations have been shown to be highly sensitive to the way in which an object is used functionally. For example, although both the words cup and bookend denote man-made tools that can be easily hand-held, cup elicits greater levels of activation in action-relevant areas than bookend, presumably because one must move a cup continuously (in contrast to a bookend) to use it functionally (Rueschemeyer et al, in press; see also Bub, Masson, & Cree, 2008;Masson, Bub, & Newton-Taylor, 2008 for converging behavioral evidence). These results show that how an object is typically manipulated is critical in determining how the lexical representation of the object is processed in the brain, and further that embodied lexical representations are quite specific in the type of experiential information they reflect.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%