2015
DOI: 10.1186/s40693-015-0036-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Evidence that the house finch (Carpodacus mexicanus) uses scent to avoid omnivore mammals

Abstract: Background: The detection of predator chemical cues is an important antipredatory behaviour as it allows an early assessment of predation risk without encountering the predator and therefore increases survival. For instance, since chemical cues are often by-products of metabolism, olfaction may gather information not only on the identity but also about the diet of predators in the vicinity. Knowledge of the role of olfaction in the interactions of birds with their environment, in contexts as important as preda… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Olfactory cues have routinely been used to assess the responses to perceived predation risk in studies on passerines. In the majority of these studies, birds respond to a predatory scent by avoidance (Amo et al 2008, 2011a, b, 2015, 2017; Mönkkönen et al 2009; but see Godard et al 2007; Johnson et al 2011). For example, breeding blue tits become increasingly vigilant, i.e., delay their entry into the nest box and decrease the time spent inside the nest box during feeding when exposed to predatory olfactory cues (ferret) (Amo et al 2008) in a largely similar manner as when exposed to visual predator cues (Amo et al 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Olfactory cues have routinely been used to assess the responses to perceived predation risk in studies on passerines. In the majority of these studies, birds respond to a predatory scent by avoidance (Amo et al 2008, 2011a, b, 2015, 2017; Mönkkönen et al 2009; but see Godard et al 2007; Johnson et al 2011). For example, breeding blue tits become increasingly vigilant, i.e., delay their entry into the nest box and decrease the time spent inside the nest box during feeding when exposed to predatory olfactory cues (ferret) (Amo et al 2008) in a largely similar manner as when exposed to visual predator cues (Amo et al 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To test whether egg covering is an antipredatory strategy in blue tit, we increased the perceived risk of predation by adding predator scent inside the nest boxes during the laying period. Many prey species can detect predator chemical cues and use them to assess the level of predation risk (Kats & Dill, ), including birds (Amo, Galván, Tomás, & Sanz, ; Amo, López‐Rull, Pagán, & Macías‐Garcia, ; Amo, Visser, & van Oers, ; Eichholz, Dassow, Stafford, & Weatherhead, ; Roth, Cox, & Lima, ; Zidar & Lovlie, ), and including blue tits (Amo et al, ). Therefore, predator chemical cues can be used to simulate an increase in the perceived risk of predation.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It was also shown that blue tits did not decrease the provisioning rate to nestlings, but they decreased the time devoted to other activities not essential for nestling survival such as nest sanitation activities [ 26 ]. Other species have also been shown to avoid predator chemical cues in a foraging context, such as the house finch, Carpodacus mexicanus [ 27 , 28 ] and the domestic fowl, Gallus gallus domesticus [ 29 ]. Birds can also avoid nesting in areas containing predator scent, as Anas sp.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%