2013
DOI: 10.1002/rrq.67
|View full text |Cite|
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Evidence for Prosody in Silent Reading

Abstract: English speakers and expressive readers emphasize new content in an ongoing discourse. Do silent readers emphasize new content in their inner voice? Because the inner voice cannot be directly observed, we borrowed the capemphasis technique (e.g., "to MAY to") from the pronunciation guides of dictionaries to elicit prosodic emphasis. Extrapolating from linguistic theories of focus prosody in spoken English, we predicted and found that silent readers in experiment 1 preferred cap-emphasized, newsworthy content (… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
16
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 87 publications
1
16
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In free-stress languages such English, Italian or Spanish, the nonlexical route should involve the use of rules not only about how to read segments, but also where to assign lexical stress, since it has been showed that readers impose lexical stress and intonation during silent reading (e.g., Ashby & Clifton, 2005;Gross, Millett, Bartek, Bredell, & Winegard, 2013), and that lexical stress plays a role in lexical access (e.g., Soto-Faraco, Sebastián-Gallés, & Cutler, 2001). Although this is not an easy task in languages with unpredictable stress (such as English or Italian), some models have successfully integrated rules to assign lexical stress (see, for example, Perry, Ziegler, & Zorzi, 2010;Rastle & Coltheart, 2000;Ševa, Monaghan, & Arciuli, 2009, for English;and Pagliuca & Monaghan, 2010;Perry, Ziegler, & Zorzi, 2014, for Italian).…”
Section: Visual Word Recognition Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In free-stress languages such English, Italian or Spanish, the nonlexical route should involve the use of rules not only about how to read segments, but also where to assign lexical stress, since it has been showed that readers impose lexical stress and intonation during silent reading (e.g., Ashby & Clifton, 2005;Gross, Millett, Bartek, Bredell, & Winegard, 2013), and that lexical stress plays a role in lexical access (e.g., Soto-Faraco, Sebastián-Gallés, & Cutler, 2001). Although this is not an easy task in languages with unpredictable stress (such as English or Italian), some models have successfully integrated rules to assign lexical stress (see, for example, Perry, Ziegler, & Zorzi, 2010;Rastle & Coltheart, 2000;Ševa, Monaghan, & Arciuli, 2009, for English;and Pagliuca & Monaghan, 2010;Perry, Ziegler, & Zorzi, 2014, for Italian).…”
Section: Visual Word Recognition Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is possible that our readers were responding to other linguistic features that systematically covaried with our experimental manipulations of prosodic meter. Nonetheless, our claims about a rhythmic inner voice are strengthened by testing specific predictions drawn from linguistic theory, by exploiting different stimuli (e.g., poems with different meters, heteronyms in prose), and by generalizing our previous research in silent reading on focus prosody (Gross et al., ) to prosodic meter and lexical stress. Third, the design of our experiments cannot tease apart interference and facilitation effects, because the experimental design lacks a plain text control condition.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Emerging evidence suggests that silent readers represent in the inner voice the prosodic features of phrasing (Bader, ; Hwang & Schafer, ), lexical stress (Ashby & Clifton, ; Ashby & Rayner, ), meter (Breen & Clifton, , ), and focal stress (Gross, Millett, Bartek, Bredell, & Winegard, ), consistent with the implicit prosody hypothesis (Fodor, ). By observing eye movements, Ashby and Clifton found that polysyllabic words with two stressed syllables were read more slowly and received more fixations than did polysyllabic words with one stressed syllable.…”
mentioning
confidence: 72%
See 2 more Smart Citations