2016
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00264.2015
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Evidence against perfect integration of sensory information during perceptual decision making

Abstract: Perceptual decision making is often modeled as perfect integration of sequential sensory samples until the accumulated total reaches a fixed decision bound. In that view, the buildup of neural activity during perceptual decision making is attributed to temporal integration. However, an alternative explanation is that sensory estimates are computed quickly with a low-pass filter and combined with a growing signal reflecting the urgency to respond and it is the latter that is primarily responsible for neural act… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
59
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
4
59
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These include the driftdiffusion model (Ratcliff, 1978;Gold and Shadlen, 2007), which assumes perfect integration of sensory evidence to a fixed accuracy criterion, as well as variations in which the accuracy criterion decreases over time (Drugowitsch et al, 2012) possibly due to a rising urgency signal (Ditterich, 2006; or in which integration is "leaky" (Usher and McClelland, 2001). One can conceive of each of these models as lying within a space defined by different assumptions about parameter settings (Thura, 2015). For example, the drift-diffusion model lies at a corner corresponding to zero leak and zero urgency, the leaky competing accumulator (Usher and McClelland, 2001) assumes leak but no urgency, the bounded accumulator with urgency (Drugowitsch et al, 2012) assumes no leak but a growing urgency, and the UGM assumes both a large leak and growing urgency (Cisek et al, 2009;.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…These include the driftdiffusion model (Ratcliff, 1978;Gold and Shadlen, 2007), which assumes perfect integration of sensory evidence to a fixed accuracy criterion, as well as variations in which the accuracy criterion decreases over time (Drugowitsch et al, 2012) possibly due to a rising urgency signal (Ditterich, 2006; or in which integration is "leaky" (Usher and McClelland, 2001). One can conceive of each of these models as lying within a space defined by different assumptions about parameter settings (Thura, 2015). For example, the drift-diffusion model lies at a corner corresponding to zero leak and zero urgency, the leaky competing accumulator (Usher and McClelland, 2001) assumes leak but no urgency, the bounded accumulator with urgency (Drugowitsch et al, 2012) assumes no leak but a growing urgency, and the UGM assumes both a large leak and growing urgency (Cisek et al, 2009;.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the UGM provides a good fit to such data as well; and indeed, Hawkins et al (2015a) showed that it fits better than perfect accumulator models without urgency to the classic data from Roitman and Shadlen (2002). Furthermore, Carland et al (2015) showed that, even during random-dot motion discrimination tasks, the motion signal is not integrated with the long time constant usually assumed by EAMs, but with a time constant on the order of 200 ms. This is consistent with the UGM and with the proposal that neural activity buildup is primarily caused by an urgency signal.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This pattern of residuals has been cast in decision-related terms as an added urgency signal that serves to push the accumulated evidence closer to the bound (Carland et al 2016, Churchland et al 2008, Cisek et al 2009, Hanks et al 2014). In LIP experiments, this urgency signal was originally defined as the residuals between the observed LIP response and the predicted time course based on symmetric drift-diffusion.…”
Section: Applying the Sensorimotor Multiplexing Perspective To Other mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In many instances, subjects have exhibited “early weighting,” where sensory evidence presented in early epochs contributes more to choices than that in late (Huk and Shadlen, 2005; Kiani et al, 2008, Nienborg and Cumming, 2009; Yates et al, 2017). In other instances, however, “late weighting” has been observed, where choices were primarily influenced by sensory evidence presented in late stimulus epochs (Tsetsos et al, 2012; Cheadle et al, 2014; Bronfman et al, 2016; Carland et al, 2016). In rodents, a mixture of either early or flat weighting profiles has been reported (Erlich et al, 2015; Scott et al, 2015; Pinto et al, 2017; Licata et al, 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%