2020
DOI: 10.1111/1467-9817.12296
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Evaluating models of how morphological awareness connects to reading comprehension: A study in Portuguese

Abstract: Background Reading comprehension is a complex skill, drawing on a range of resources. One is morphological awareness, or the awareness of the smallest meaningful units in language. Testing the predictions of the Simple View of Reading, we evaluate how morphological awareness relates to reading comprehension in a relatively transparent orthography. Method There were 70 Portuguese‐speaking children in Grade 2 in our study. The children completed measures of morphological awareness and reading comprehension, alon… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
10
0
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
0
10
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These include languages represented with alphabets that are highly phonologically transparent, such as Finnish (e.g., Bertram et al, 2000;Lehtonen & Bryant, 2005), Spanish (e.g., D'Alessio et al, 2018;Suárez-Coalla et al, 2017), and Italian (e.g., Angelelli et al, 2017) and those that are less so, such as French (Quémart et al, 2011). This extends to languages with more complex syllable structure, such as German (e.g., Hasenäcker et al, 2017) and those less so, such as Greek (e.g., Diamanti et al 2017;Manolitsis et al, 2017) and Portuguese (e.g., Oliveira et al, 2020). It also extends to languages represented with abjads, such as Hebrew (e.g., Schiff et al, 2012), and those with morphosyllabic writing systems, such as Chinese (e.g., Tong et al, 2017).…”
Section: The Morphological Pathways Framework Beyond Englishmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These include languages represented with alphabets that are highly phonologically transparent, such as Finnish (e.g., Bertram et al, 2000;Lehtonen & Bryant, 2005), Spanish (e.g., D'Alessio et al, 2018;Suárez-Coalla et al, 2017), and Italian (e.g., Angelelli et al, 2017) and those that are less so, such as French (Quémart et al, 2011). This extends to languages with more complex syllable structure, such as German (e.g., Hasenäcker et al, 2017) and those less so, such as Greek (e.g., Diamanti et al 2017;Manolitsis et al, 2017) and Portuguese (e.g., Oliveira et al, 2020). It also extends to languages represented with abjads, such as Hebrew (e.g., Schiff et al, 2012), and those with morphosyllabic writing systems, such as Chinese (e.g., Tong et al, 2017).…”
Section: The Morphological Pathways Framework Beyond Englishmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…And yet, effects of morphological processing on literacy skills may be observable in younger children for transparent orthographies where grapheme-phoneme correspondences are mastered earlier in development than in English (Hasenäcker et al, 2017;Lehtonen & Bryant, 2005). Relatedly, questions remain in transparent orthographies as to whether morphology influences reading and spelling accuracy (e.g., Angelelli et al, 2017;Oliveira et al, 2020) or whether effects are more clearly observed on fluency (e.g., Burani et al, 2008;D'Alessio et al, 2018;Marcolini et al, 2011; but see Diamanti et al, 2017;Manolitsis et al, 2017). Further still, the division of labor between morpho-orthographic and morpho-semantic segmentation is a likely candidate for cross-linguistic differences, with unanswered questions pertaining to how much the semantic properties of morphemes support reading and spelling in transparent versus opaque languages across development (e.g., Beyersmann et al, 2012;Feldman et al, 2009;Schiff et al, 2012).…”
Section: The Morphological Pathways Framework Beyond Englishmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These include languages represented with alphabets that are highly phonologically transparent, such as Finnish (e.g., Bertram, Laine, & Virkkala, 2000;Lehtonen & Bryant, 2005), Spanish (e.g., D'Alessio, Jaichenco, & Wilson, 2018;Suárez-Coalla, Martínez-García, & Cuetos, 2017) and Italian (e.g., Angelelli, Marinelli, De Salvatore, & Burani, 2017), and those that are less so, such as French (Quémart et al, 2011). This extends to languages with more complex syllable structure, such as German (e.g., Hasenäcker, Schröter, & Schroeder, 2017), and those less so, such as Greek (e.g., Diamanti et al, 2017;Manolitsis, Grigorakis, & Georgiou, 2017) and Portuguese (e.g., Oliveira, Levesque, Deacon, & da Mota, 2020). It also extends to languages represented with abjads, such as Hebrew (e.g., Schiff et al, 2012), and those with morphosyllabic writing systems, such as Chinese (e.g., Tong, Tong, & McBride, 2017).…”
Section: The Morphological Pathways Framework Beyond Englishmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…And yet effects of morphological processing on literacy skills may be observable in younger children for transparent orthographies where grapheme–phoneme correspondences are mastered earlier in development than in English (Hasenäcker et al, 2017; Lehtonen & Bryant, 2005). Relatedly, questions remain in transparent orthographies as to whether morphology influences reading and spelling accuracy (e.g., Angelelli et al, 2017; Oliveira et al, 2020) or whether effects are more clearly observed on fluency (e.g., Burani, Marcolini, De Luca, & Zoccolotti, 2008; D'Alessio et al, 2018; Marcolini, Traficante, Zoccolotti, & Burani, 2011; but see Diamanti et al, 2017; Manolitsis et al, 2017). Further still, the division of labour between morpho‐orthographic and morpho‐semantic segmentation is a likely candidate for cross‐linguistic differences, with unanswered questions pertaining to how much the semantic properties of morphemes support reading and spelling in transparent versus opaque languages across development (e.g., Beyersmann et al, 2012; Feldman, O'Connor, & del Prado Martín, 2009; Schiff et al, 2012).…”
Section: The Morphological Pathways Framework Beyond Englishmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast to Chinese, Spanish predominantly uses derivational affixes to create multimorphemic words such as person+al+ity (personalidad). Sensitivity to derivational morphology aids children's recognition of long, polysyllabic words, and may even serve as a mechanism for children with reading difficulties (Suárez-Coalla & Cuetos, 2013), both in Spanish and other closely related, derivationally rich languages such as Portuguese (Oliveira et al, 2020).…”
Section: Study 2: Bilingual Transfer Effects On Morphological Awareness and English Readingmentioning
confidence: 99%