2001
DOI: 10.1162/089976601750541804
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Evaluating Auditory Performance Limits: I. One-Parameter Discrimination Using a Computational Model for the Auditory Nerve

Abstract: A method for calculating psychophysical performance limits based on stochastic neural responses is introduced and compared to previous analytical methods for evaluating auditory discrimination of tone frequency and level. The method uses signal detection theory and a computational model for a population of auditory nerve (AN) fiber responses. The use of computational models allows predictions to be made over a wider parameter range and with more complete descriptions of AN responses than in analytical models. … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

13
189
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 169 publications
(202 citation statements)
references
References 95 publications
13
189
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, the neural bases for human perceptual abilities, such as frequency or vowel discrimination, over a large range of intensities are poorly understood and much debated (45,46), but quantitative models are invariably based on the frequency selectivity measured in other animals. The debate as to whether cochlear frequency selectivity is insufficiently sharp and needs to be complemented with a temporal code to operate over the greater than 100-dB range of intensities over which humans hear is fundamental to the understanding of the effects of cochlear pathology, and thus for developing algorithms for auditory prostheses to relieve hearing loss and deafness.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, the neural bases for human perceptual abilities, such as frequency or vowel discrimination, over a large range of intensities are poorly understood and much debated (45,46), but quantitative models are invariably based on the frequency selectivity measured in other animals. The debate as to whether cochlear frequency selectivity is insufficiently sharp and needs to be complemented with a temporal code to operate over the greater than 100-dB range of intensities over which humans hear is fundamental to the understanding of the effects of cochlear pathology, and thus for developing algorithms for auditory prostheses to relieve hearing loss and deafness.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(1)-(3) and using the description of sensitivity based on a single AN fiber rate function [e.g., Siebert (1970); see Heinz et al (2001a) for derivation], the normalized sensitivity squared for the AN model population is [Eq. 5.34 in Heinz (2000)] (4) where M is the total number of fibers in the population; r i (t|f,w) is the response of the ith AN model fiber to the wth vowel-plus-masker with the second formant frequency, f. T is the duration of the stimulus waveform.…”
Section: Threshold Predictions Based On An Model Responsesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Siebert (1965Siebert ( ,1968) developed a strategy for predicting limits of level discrimination for pure tones based on a very simple description of the auditory periphery. Heinz et al (2001a) extended Siebert's approach to allow the use of recent computational models for AN responses. Since both approaches include analyzing population responses and combining information across fibers tuned to different frequencies, they provide appropriate tools to study formant-frequency encoding.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The tonotopic arrangement of hair cells and primary afferents allows sound frequency to be encoded using the number of spikes by a rate-place code. Alternatively, some sound frequencies can be encoded using the precise timing of spikes by phase-locking to the stimulus waveform (Kiang, 1965;Rose et al, 1967;Heinz et al, 2001). Furthermore, firing rate, synchronization, and phase cues can all encode changes in sound level (Kiang, 1965;Anderson et al, 1971;Johnson, 1980;Colburn et al, 2003).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, firing rate, synchronization, and phase cues can all encode changes in sound level (Kiang, 1965;Anderson et al, 1971;Johnson, 1980;Colburn et al, 2003). As in all other sensory systems, trial-to-trial variability in spike count and timing imposes limitations on discrimination performance (Heinz et al, 2001).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%