2015
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-3106a
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Ethics Rounds Needs to Consider Evidence for Listening and Spoken Language for Deaf Children

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

3
2
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 2 publications
3
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…reported that more than 90% of families at the Ontario Infant Hearing Program in Canada chose an LSL communication option. Similar findings are reported by Sugar and Goldberg (2015), where more than 88% of families chose an LSL outcome for their children in Ohio, United States (US). This evidence is important as it affirms that when hearing-impaired children receive appropriate EI services delivered by well-trained and experienced professionals utilising their chosen communication approach, they exhibit typical developmental milestones in speech, language and scholastic outcomes (Houston & Stredler-Brown 2012).…”
Section: Communication Therapeutic Approachessupporting
confidence: 82%
“…reported that more than 90% of families at the Ontario Infant Hearing Program in Canada chose an LSL communication option. Similar findings are reported by Sugar and Goldberg (2015), where more than 88% of families chose an LSL outcome for their children in Ohio, United States (US). This evidence is important as it affirms that when hearing-impaired children receive appropriate EI services delivered by well-trained and experienced professionals utilising their chosen communication approach, they exhibit typical developmental milestones in speech, language and scholastic outcomes (Houston & Stredler-Brown 2012).…”
Section: Communication Therapeutic Approachessupporting
confidence: 82%
“…Unfortunately, just as the term language is often taken to refer to spoken language alone, some have mistakenly believed the critical period applies only to spoken languages. The baseless idea that there is a longer critical period for sign language acquisition has been used to argue that spoken language exposure must be prioritized at the expense of sign language exposure (Sugar & Goldberg, 2015). In contrast, research with the sign-language-as-a-fallback option reveals that deaf adults who did not have access to or achieved age-appropriate mastery of a sign language in childhood do not ever achieve such fluency in their lifetime (Cheng, Halgren, & Mayberry, 2018; Emmorey, 2018; Mayberry, 2010; Mayberry, Chen, Witcher, & Klein, 2011; Mayberry, Davenport, Roth, & Halgren, 2018; Mayberry & Eichen, 1991; Mayberry & Lock, 2003; Newport, 1990; Skotara, Salden, Kügow, Hänel-Faulhaber, & Röder, 2012; Woll, 2018).…”
Section: Theoretical and Practical Arguments Against Natural Sign Lanmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some hearing loss professionals and organizations advocate for preventing sign language exposure through the Listening and Spoken Language approach, what is historically known as oralism (Sugar, 2016; Sugar & Goldberg, 2015). This opposition to sign language is not based on empirical evidence supporting the harm of sign language exposure, thus perpetuating misinformation such as the sign language acquisition window being longer than the spoken language window.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%