2007
DOI: 10.1007/s00265-006-0287-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Estrus cycle asynchrony in wild female chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
28
1
1

Year Published

2008
2008
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
28
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Such an effect was reported for humans (Stern and McClintock, 1998) and has been inferred in earlier studies reporting synchrony in humans (Weller and Weller, 1993). However, other studies or re-analysis of previous studies, have found no evidence of cycle synchrony in humans (Schank, 2001b; Strassmann, 1999; Whitten, 1999; Wilson, 1987, 1992), non-human primates (chimpanzee: (Matsumoto-Oda et al, 2007); Mandrill: (Setchell et al, 2011); macaque (Furtbauer et al, 2011)), rats (Schank, 2001a) or hamsters (Schank, 2000). It might be that cycle synchrony occurs only in very restricted and contextually-specific circumstances (McClintock, 2002) but, on balance, the evidence suggests that ovarian synchrony does not exist as a biologically meaningful phenomenon (Schank, 2001b, 2006).…”
Section: Ovulation Induction (Whitten Effect; (Whitten Et Al 1968))mentioning
confidence: 85%
“…Such an effect was reported for humans (Stern and McClintock, 1998) and has been inferred in earlier studies reporting synchrony in humans (Weller and Weller, 1993). However, other studies or re-analysis of previous studies, have found no evidence of cycle synchrony in humans (Schank, 2001b; Strassmann, 1999; Whitten, 1999; Wilson, 1987, 1992), non-human primates (chimpanzee: (Matsumoto-Oda et al, 2007); Mandrill: (Setchell et al, 2011); macaque (Furtbauer et al, 2011)), rats (Schank, 2001a) or hamsters (Schank, 2000). It might be that cycle synchrony occurs only in very restricted and contextually-specific circumstances (McClintock, 2002) but, on balance, the evidence suggests that ovarian synchrony does not exist as a biologically meaningful phenomenon (Schank, 2001b, 2006).…”
Section: Ovulation Induction (Whitten Effect; (Whitten Et Al 1968))mentioning
confidence: 85%
“…Since female‐female aggression correlates with the number of females in oestrus, but not with the number of males in the group, this suggests that female bonobos may compete for sperm from specific males. In chimpanzees, there is also evidence that females who harass mating pairs are themselves in oestrus (Nishida, 1979; Niemeyer & Anderson, 1983); moreover, female chimpanzees apparently avoid synchronizing their oestrus, which may be an adaptation to reduce competition for limited sperm (Matsumoto‐Oda et al , 2007). However, it is important to note that although competition for sperm may well explain female behaviour in these examples, other explanations cannot necessarily be ruled out.…”
Section: Why Do Female Mammals Compete For Access To Mates?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some suggested benefits of promiscuity are confusing paternity and avoiding infanticide (Hrdy 1979;van Noordwijk and van Schaik 2000; van Schaik et al 2000), enhancing high-quality genes through male-male and sperm competition (Clutton-Brock and Harvey 1976;Edvardsson et al 2007), and increasing the probability of fertilization (Burley 1989;Small 1990). Costs of promiscuity are energy and time (Beach 1976;Matsumoto-Oda and Oda 1998;Wrangham 1979), female-female competition (Matsumoto-Oda et al 2007), and male sexual coercion (Muller et al 2007;Smuts and Smuts 1993).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 97%