2016
DOI: 10.1086/687254
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Escape Distance in Ground-Nesting Birds Differs with Individual Level of Camouflage

Abstract: Camouflage is one of the most widespread anti-predator strategies in the animal kingdom, yet no animal can match its background perfectly in a complex environment. Therefore, selection should favour individuals that use information on how effective their camouflage is in their immediate habitat when responding to an approaching threat. In a field study of African ground-nesting birds (plovers, coursers, and nightjars), we tested the hypothesis that individuals adaptively modulate their escape behaviour in rela… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
62
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(69 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
6
62
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Furthermore, grasshoppers showed threat‐dependent behaviour in line with their camouflage level, in that individuals with better camouflage allowed predators to come closer before escaping. Similar findings have been found in ground‐nesting birds, whereby flight‐initiation distances from a nest when a threat approaches vary with levels of egg or adult camouflage (Wilson‐Aggarwal et al, ).…”
Section: Background Choicesupporting
confidence: 76%
“…Furthermore, grasshoppers showed threat‐dependent behaviour in line with their camouflage level, in that individuals with better camouflage allowed predators to come closer before escaping. Similar findings have been found in ground‐nesting birds, whereby flight‐initiation distances from a nest when a threat approaches vary with levels of egg or adult camouflage (Wilson‐Aggarwal et al, ).…”
Section: Background Choicesupporting
confidence: 76%
“…A good approach is to analyse the overlap (in a species‐specific colour space) of the distributions of all colours in the objects being compared (Endler & Mielke, ), or the size distribution and spatial adjacency of colour patches (Endler, , ). Better still, from the perspective taken in this review, is to decompose the patterns with respect to their different spatial frequency components, which can then be used to assess similarity at coarse‐ to fine‐grained detail, or to produce an aggregate measure of ‘match’ (the method used by Troscianko et al ., ; Wilson‐Aggarwal et al ., ). This is an appealing approach because this is similar to what happens in the initial stages of visual processing (Troscianko et al ., ; Osorio & Cuthill, ; Merilaita et al ., ).…”
Section: Peeling the Onionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Many of the empirical studies that demonstrate a camouflage function of patterns in non-colour changing animals show an association in one or a limited number of species (e.g., Lovell et al, 2013; Kang et al, 2014; Marshall, Philpot & Stevens, 2015b.; Wilson-Aggarwal et al, 2016) or were found via predator–prey computer simulations (e.g., Stevens, Yule & Ruxton, 2008; Stevens et al, 2011; Scott-Samuel et al, 2011; Troscianko et al, 2013; How & Zanker, 2014; Hughes, Troscianko & Stevens, 2014; reviewed in Marshall & Gluckman, 2015). At the level of microhabitats, some studies demonstrate that individual behaviours may facilitate camouflage, such as a behavioural choice to rest on backgrounds that enhance camouflage (Tsurui, Honma & Nishida, 2010; Lovell et al, 2013; Kang et al, 2014; Marshall, Philpot & Stevens, 2015b.; Troscianko et al, 2016; Wilson-Aggarwal et al, 2016). Under current camouflage theory, microhabitat usage in closed and open habitats should result in irregular patterning being associated with all habitats, as it is typically invoked in static camouflage.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%