1978
DOI: 10.3758/bf03198242
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Error processes in syllogistic reasoning

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
17
0
1

Year Published

1982
1982
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
1
17
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Since the arrangement of propositions affects syllogism difficulty (cf. Dickstein, 1978;Kaufmann and Goldstein, 1967;Roberge, 1970;Woodworth and Sells, 1935), two moderately difficult arrangements of propositions were used to an equal degree in constructing pro-union, anti-union, and neutral syllogism (cf. Evans et al, 1983)'.…”
Section: Plausibility Scalementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Since the arrangement of propositions affects syllogism difficulty (cf. Dickstein, 1978;Kaufmann and Goldstein, 1967;Roberge, 1970;Woodworth and Sells, 1935), two moderately difficult arrangements of propositions were used to an equal degree in constructing pro-union, anti-union, and neutral syllogism (cf. Evans et al, 1983)'.…”
Section: Plausibility Scalementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In some cases, individuals respond with a conclusion that fits the atmosphere, but in many cases, they correctly respond that "nothing follows"-a phenomenon that the atmosphere hypothesis cannot explain, because there is always a conclusion that fits the atmosphere of the premises. Conversely, there are cases in which individuals fail to draw a valid conclusion and respond wrongly that nothing follows even though the valid conclusion fits the atmosphere (Johnson-Laird & Bara, 1984b, p. 7; see also Dickstein, 1978;Mazzocco, Legrenzi, & Roncato, 1974).…”
Section: Heuristic Theories Of Syllogistic Reasoningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We likewise searched for dissertations, book chapters, and unpublished articles by consulting both reference sections and experts in the field. We excluded studies that did not examine all 64 pairs of premises or that examined only syllogisms with conclusions constrained to the Scholastic order C-A (e.g., Dickstein, 1978;and Rips, 1994, p. 233).…”
Section: The Literature Search and Effect Sizementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In some experiments [1], [2], [4], [5], choice options were given to participants as instantiated above, while in other experiments [3], [6], [7], [8], participants were asked to generate their own conclusions.…”
Section: Experimental Design Materials and Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Table 1, Table 2 show the comparison of the probabilistic representation theory (PRT) [1] with other models (the transitive-chain theory (TCT) [2] and the probability heuristic model (PHM) [3], respectively). Table 3, Table 4, Table 5, Table 6, Table 7, Table 8, Table 9 show the comparison of three models (i.e., PRT, PHM, and a probabilistic extension of the mental model theory [pMM] [1], [4]) using data of Experiment 1 in [5], Experiment 2 in [5], Experiment 2, first Test in [4], Experiment 2, second Test in [4], Experiment 1 in [2], Experiment 3 in [6], Experiment with adult participants in [7], Experiment in [8], Experiment 1 in [1], and Experiment 2 in [1], respectively. Tables 13 and 14 show the comparison of PRT and PHM using data from syllogisms with generalized quantifiers, Experiment 1 in [3] and Experiment 2 in [3], respectively.…”
Section: Datamentioning
confidence: 99%