2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Enhanced Avoidance Habits in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder

Abstract: BackgroundObsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is a psychiatric condition that typically manifests in compulsive urges to perform irrational or excessive avoidance behaviors. A recent account has suggested that compulsivity in OCD might arise from excessive stimulus-response habit formation, rendering behavior insensitive to goal value. We tested if OCD patients have a bias toward habits using a novel shock avoidance task. To explore how habits, as a putative model of compulsivity, might relate to obsessions an… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

24
340
6
3

Year Published

2014
2014
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 342 publications
(373 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
24
340
6
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Using an aversive shock habit task, OCD subjects showed greater habitual responding following overtraining to a virtual devaluation of a shock outcome (43). These results contrast with a study using a loss version of the twostep task where OCD subjects demonstrated the opposite, with more pronounced model-based behaviors to avoid loss outcomes (42).…”
Section: A C C E P T E D Accepted Manuscriptcontrasting
confidence: 80%
“…Using an aversive shock habit task, OCD subjects showed greater habitual responding following overtraining to a virtual devaluation of a shock outcome (43). These results contrast with a study using a loss version of the twostep task where OCD subjects demonstrated the opposite, with more pronounced model-based behaviors to avoid loss outcomes (42).…”
Section: A C C E P T E D Accepted Manuscriptcontrasting
confidence: 80%
“…Along the same lines, amphetamine pre-treatment speeds up the rate at which the outcome insensitivity develops (Nelson and Killcross, 2006), and this depends particularly on D 1 rather than D 2 receptors (Nelson and Killcross, 2013). In humans, there is evidence for enhanced habitization in obsessive-compulsive disorder (Everitt and Robbins, 2005;Robbins et al, 2012;Gillan et al, 2011Gillan et al, , 2013 and forthcoming evidence in cocaine addiction (N. Daw and V. Voon, personal communication), but not yet in alcohol addiction (Sebold et al, subm).…”
Section: Individual Variability In Addiction Vulnerabilitymentioning
confidence: 96%
“…It is important, though, to bear in mind that although habits share features with compulsions (Gillan et al, 2011(Gillan et al, , 2013, they are not one and the same (Dayan 2009;Robbins et al 2012 and many others). It has been suggested that after extended training, habits become deeply engrained by shifting further dorsally in the corticostriatal loops (Belin and Everitt, 2008;Willuhn et al, 2012).…”
Section: Shifts Towards Model-free Learning In Addictionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…After an outcome devaluation (e.g., through satiation), the MB system can change preferences quickly, but the MF system cannot. Individual variation in the balance between MB and MF decisions, with a shift toward MF and away from MB learning, is associated with addictive and impulsive traits in animals (Huys et al, 2014;Everitt & Robbins, 2005), and a bias has been reported in conditions such as addiction and obsessive-compulsive disorder where behavioral preferences persist against explicit desires (Voon et al, , 2015Gillan et al, 2011Gillan et al, , 2014Sebold et al, 2014;Sjoerds et al, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%