2006
DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.02.008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Encountering the expertise reversal effect with a computer-based environment on electrical circuit analysis

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
101
1
2

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 107 publications
(113 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
(30 reference statements)
9
101
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The ability to do so seems to be related to one's knowledge of the tasks (Dunning et al 2003), which novices lack. This fits with the findings of Reisslein et al (2006), who found an interaction of example-problem pairs and problem-example pairs with learners' prior knowledge: Whereas low prior knowledge learners benefited most from example-problem pairs, high prior knowledge learners benefited most from problem-example pairs. In tutored problem solving, however, learners can request help immediately when they experience that they cannot solve a particular step, which is unproblematic for novices and, in this case, help consisting of annotated worked examples has been shown to be more efficient (equal test performance achieved in less learning time) than help consisting of hints (Ringenberg and VanLehn 2006).…”
Section: How Should Examples Be Delivered To Optimize Their Effectivesupporting
confidence: 89%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The ability to do so seems to be related to one's knowledge of the tasks (Dunning et al 2003), which novices lack. This fits with the findings of Reisslein et al (2006), who found an interaction of example-problem pairs and problem-example pairs with learners' prior knowledge: Whereas low prior knowledge learners benefited most from example-problem pairs, high prior knowledge learners benefited most from problem-example pairs. In tutored problem solving, however, learners can request help immediately when they experience that they cannot solve a particular step, which is unproblematic for novices and, in this case, help consisting of annotated worked examples has been shown to be more efficient (equal test performance achieved in less learning time) than help consisting of hints (Ringenberg and VanLehn 2006).…”
Section: How Should Examples Be Delivered To Optimize Their Effectivesupporting
confidence: 89%
“…Reisslein et al (2006) found no overall differences between fading, problem-example pairs, and example-problem pairs, but Renkl et al (2002) and Atkinson et al (2003b) did find a fading strategy to be more effective than example-problem pairs, especially backward fading in which the last solution steps are omitted first in the completion problems.…”
Section: How Should Examples Be Delivered To Optimize Their Effectivementioning
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Then, when applying the rubrics, teachers might have encountered redundant, extraneous, or even conflicting information within the rubric. This, in turn, may have caused extensive cognitive load and thus did not benefit or even hindered rubric application (Kalyuga et al, 2003;Kalyuga & Renkl, 2010;Reisslein et al, 2006). In contrast, these rubrics may have provided important scaffolding for non-teachers, for example essential information about OER use and application, which supported non-teachers in completing the evaluation tasks (Kalyuga et al, 2003).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research within the cognitive load framework has identified the "expertise reversal effect," showing that instructional tools that benefit novices may not necessarily benefit or can even be detrimental to experts (Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller, 2003;Kalyuga & Renkl, 2010). For example, Reisslein, Atkinson, Seeling, and Reisslein (2006) exposed students to a series of instructional procedures in a computer-based environment and found that detailed instructions with examples were beneficial for inexperienced learners, but less so for experienced learners. Similarly, Oksa, Kalyuga, and Chandler (2010) found that when using the same explanatory scaffolds, people with low prior knowledge outperformed the control group in understanding target content, while people with high prior knowledge performed worse than the control group.…”
Section: User Characteristics Associated With Rubric Usementioning
confidence: 99%