1970
DOI: 10.1037/h0028569
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Encoding categories of words: An empirical approach to meaning.

Abstract: This article reports a series of studies investigating the dimension along which words are encoded, using the "release from proactive inhibition" in short-term memory technique. The results of the experiments indicate that different dimensions vary in their effectiveness for proactive inhibition release. In general, semantic dimensions (taxonomic categories or semantic differential) are highly effective, whereas physical characteristics such as word length or figure-ground colors of the slide presentation are … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

20
411
3
3

Year Published

1991
1991
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 628 publications
(437 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
20
411
3
3
Order By: Relevance
“…A mixed 3 × 2 × 2 ANOVA was conducted on number recalled, with list (1, 2, 3) as a within-subject factor, and age (older, younger) and time of testing (TOD; morning vs. afternoon) as between-subject factors. As is commonly found in this literature (e.g., Wickens, 1972), recall declined across the lists, F(2, 184) = 37.471, MSE = 1.95, p < 0.05, indicating the buildup of PI. No other effects involving List (List × Age, List × TOD, and List × Age × TOD, largest F < 1) were significant, suggesting that the declines across lists did not differ across age and between people tested at their optimal and nonoptimal times of day.…”
Section: Pi Buildup: Recall Across Lists and Intrusion Errorssupporting
confidence: 73%
“…A mixed 3 × 2 × 2 ANOVA was conducted on number recalled, with list (1, 2, 3) as a within-subject factor, and age (older, younger) and time of testing (TOD; morning vs. afternoon) as between-subject factors. As is commonly found in this literature (e.g., Wickens, 1972), recall declined across the lists, F(2, 184) = 37.471, MSE = 1.95, p < 0.05, indicating the buildup of PI. No other effects involving List (List × Age, List × TOD, and List × Age × TOD, largest F < 1) were significant, suggesting that the declines across lists did not differ across age and between people tested at their optimal and nonoptimal times of day.…”
Section: Pi Buildup: Recall Across Lists and Intrusion Errorssupporting
confidence: 73%
“…Even when these frontal patients were matched on List 1 recall to patients with nonfrontal lesions (or to healthy controls), the frontal patients had more difficulty recalling the later word lists. Interference-buildup tasks typically produce highly variable data and require large samples to detect group interactions (see, e.g., Wickens, 1970). Thus, these findings of significant frontal deficits with modest-sized patient groups are particularly noteworthy.…”
Section: Interference In Selective Attention Memory Retrieval and Smentioning
confidence: 86%
“…Because identifying the primary conceptual dimensions is far more difficult in these domains, can graded effects be observed? To date little empirical work has addressed this question (beyond studies using the "release from proactive interference" paradigm 1 ; Wickens, 1970;Wickens, Dalezman, & Eggemeier, 1963), although a number of different theories of meaning representation predict semantic distance effects, especially those assuming distributed semantic representations and featural overlap between them (e.g. Martin & Chao, 2001;McRae, de Sa, & Seidenberg, 1997;Plaut, 1995).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%