2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.111936
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

En route to delineating hippocampal roles in spatial learning

Abstract: The full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-prot purposes provided that: • a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source • a link is made to the metadata record in DRO • the full-text is not changed in any way The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 67 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This distinction has been realised by a recent virtual reality experiment in humans, showing that memory recall for words was better when participants actively explored a novel virtual environment, as opposed to passively experiencing the input of another participant (Schomaker & Wittmann, 2021). In a similar vein, passive transport training of hippocampal-lesioned and sham rats in a Morris water maze task led the control group to perform worse than the lesioned group on probe-trials when rats had to actively swim to the goal location (Poulter et al, 2019). This is echoed neurally, as when rats were passively transported in a car instead of self-generated movement, their place cell activity was degraded in number and resolution (Terrazas et al, 2005).…”
Section: Short Timescalesmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…This distinction has been realised by a recent virtual reality experiment in humans, showing that memory recall for words was better when participants actively explored a novel virtual environment, as opposed to passively experiencing the input of another participant (Schomaker & Wittmann, 2021). In a similar vein, passive transport training of hippocampal-lesioned and sham rats in a Morris water maze task led the control group to perform worse than the lesioned group on probe-trials when rats had to actively swim to the goal location (Poulter et al, 2019). This is echoed neurally, as when rats were passively transported in a car instead of self-generated movement, their place cell activity was degraded in number and resolution (Terrazas et al, 2005).…”
Section: Short Timescalesmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…But they are at odds with the notion of separate memory systems that operate in parallel, because the rules of operation for each system seem to be domain-general rather than domain-specific (Heyes, 2003). Indeed, associative competition between spatial memory systems at the level of learning has been demonstrated both by the failure of cognitive mapping when landmark vectors are particularly salient, and by the facilitation of learning governed by one system when the other is lesioned (Kosaki et al, 2015;Poulter et al, 2019; see also Gibson & Shettleworth, 2005). Further research, in addition to the purely behavioural studies we report here, and those of Mou and colleagues discussed above, will be required to test the possibility that functionally distinctive brain systems compete for control over learning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To discourage learning based on a fixed egocentric route, which could interfere with mapping of the environment (Morris, 1981;Poulter et al, 2019;Whishaw & Mittleman, 1986), participants in both groups began Stage 1 and 2 training trials from one of 4 locations, facing in a randomised direction. For the Boundary Stable group, when locating a given goal, participants began one trial from each of the four different start locations for each landmark position described previously.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Moreover, as pointed out by Andre et al (2019) , “the distinct constellation of brain structures may participate to the space representation differently depending on the navigation strategy employed, the availability of sensory information and the phase of memory utilisation” (see also Sirota and Buzsàki, 2005 ). For example, in many studies the hippocampus has been shown to be a key player in processing allocentric information and spatial mapping of the environment, but recent studies have also revealed the participation of this structure in the control of self-generated motion ( Poulter et al, 2019 ). Successful investigation of the dynamic pattern of brain regions and neurotransmitter circuit activity during the formation of unified space representation requires the identification of relatively distinct temporal stages of learning processes in the MWM.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%