2016
DOI: 10.1080/17459737.2016.1152517
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Empirically testingTonnetz, voice-leading, and spectral models of perceived triadic distance

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
31
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
0
31
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Similar to previous literature (e.g. Hutchinson & Knopoff, 1978;Milne & Holland, 2016;Parncutt & Strasburger, 1994), we default to modelling each complex tone with 11 partials, and give the th partial an amplitude of 1/ , with the amplitudes being provided in arbitrary units. These defaults are somewhat arbitrary, and the researcher is encouraged to adjust the number of harmonics and their amplitudes to reflect the particular sound being modelled.…”
Section: Sparse Frequency Spectrummentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similar to previous literature (e.g. Hutchinson & Knopoff, 1978;Milne & Holland, 2016;Parncutt & Strasburger, 1994), we default to modelling each complex tone with 11 partials, and give the th partial an amplitude of 1/ , with the amplitudes being provided in arbitrary units. These defaults are somewhat arbitrary, and the researcher is encouraged to adjust the number of harmonics and their amplitudes to reflect the particular sound being modelled.…”
Section: Sparse Frequency Spectrummentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These algorithms have three free parameters: the number of harmonics modeled in each complex tone, the harmonic roll-off rate (ρ), and the standard deviation of the Gaussian smoothing distribution (σ). We set the number of harmonics to 11 (including the fundamental frequency), and set the other two parameters to the optimized values in Milne and Holland (2016): a roll-off of ρ = 0.75, and a standard deviation of σ = 6.83 cents. (2006), whose description includes a parametric approximation for the relationship between interval size and pure-dyad dissonance (see also Bigand et al, 1996).…”
Section: Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It avoids much of the complexity of Leman's (2000) model: it has no explicit modeling of the peripheral auditory system and does not model the time-course of echoic memory. Nonetheless, the model has demonstrated best-in-class results in modeling certain important results from the psychological literature (Milne & Holland, 2016;Milne, Laney, & Sharp, 2015). Milne et al's (2011) model estimates the perceptual dissimilarity of pairs of pitch or pitch-class sets.…”
Section: Milne Et Al's (2011) Spectral Distance Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It combines each harmonic series implied by every pitch(-class), smooths the resulting spectra to account for perceptual imprecision, and then computes the cosine distance between these spectra. This cosine distance has been shown to predict perceptual 1 http://www.ipem.ugent.be/Toolbox judgements of triadic similarity rather effectively (Milne & Holland, 2016). We applied this model to harmonic expectation by using it to model the perceptual dissimilarity of a chord and its context.…”
Section: Milne Et Al's (2011) Spectral Distance Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation