2003
DOI: 10.1016/s0304-3959(03)00298-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Emotion induction moderates effects of anger management style on acute pain sensitivity

Abstract: Anger management style (AMS) is related to both acute and chronic pain intensity. Recent work suggests that an anger expressive AMS in particular may influence acute pain, and that this effect may be most pronounced during anger provocation. The present study examined whether AMS was related to subsequent pain sensitivity without regard to prior emotion induction, only when a strong negative emotion was evoked, or only when anger was provoked. Sixty-four healthy normals partook in semi-structured interviews in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
58
3
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 56 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
3
58
3
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The pain-related effects of anger-in were due to the fact that those exhibiting an anger suppressive style tend to be more affectively distressed (depressed, anxious, and angry), which in turn negatively influences pain responsiveness. This finding supports prior work suggesting that statistical overlap between anger-in and general negative affect may account for many of the reported health-related effects of anger-in [9,15,36]. In contrast, while anger-out was significantly correlated with negative affect in this study, this overlap did not account for its genetically-moderated influence on pain responsiveness.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…The pain-related effects of anger-in were due to the fact that those exhibiting an anger suppressive style tend to be more affectively distressed (depressed, anxious, and angry), which in turn negatively influences pain responsiveness. This finding supports prior work suggesting that statistical overlap between anger-in and general negative affect may account for many of the reported health-related effects of anger-in [9,15,36]. In contrast, while anger-out was significantly correlated with negative affect in this study, this overlap did not account for its genetically-moderated influence on pain responsiveness.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…For chronic low back pain patients, high levels of resting lower paraspinal muscle tension or large increases in lower paraspinal tension during emotional arousal appear to impact chronic pain severity (Burns et al 2003;Burns 2006b). However, Pain Catastrophizing Scale scores were not correlated significantly with resting muscle tension or with reactivity, and so further tests of mediation were inappropriate.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The anger and sadness recall procedures elicit significant muscle tension, SBP, DBP and HR reactivity (Burns et al 1997;Burns 2006b). Moreover, findings tell that both anger and sadness recall interviews elicit self-reported valence and arousal in expected directions (Burns et al 2003;Neumann and Waldstein 2001).Details of the procedures can be found in Burns (2006b). In brief, the experimenter asked a participant to recall a recent event in their life during which they experienced the relevant emotion.…”
mentioning
confidence: 87%
“…Both anger expression and anger suppression have been linked with pain severity via various physiological mechanisms (e.g. Bruehl, Chung, Burns, & Biridepalli, 2003;Burns, Kubilus, & Bruehl, 2003;Burns, Quartana, & Bruehl, 2008). Fear of pain has been shown to be closely related to various measures of patient functioning in chronic pain (Crombez, Vlaeyen, Heuts, & Lysens, 1999;McCracken, Zayfert, & Gross, 1992), as has anxiety sensitivity (Keogh & Cochrane, 2002) and worry (Eccleston & Crombez, 1999).…”
Section: Key Practitioner Messagementioning
confidence: 99%