1986
DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.115.1.26
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Elaboration, organization, and the self-reference effect in memory.

Abstract: Relating information to the self (self-referent encoding) has been shown to produce better recall than purely semantic encoding. This finding has been interpreted as demonstrating that self-reference produces a more elaborate memory trace than semantic encoding, and it has been cited frequently as evidence that the self is one of the most highly elaborated structures in memory. The experiments reported in this article challenge this interpretation of the self-reference effect by demonstrating that self-referen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

12
278
1
1

Year Published

1992
1992
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 253 publications
(292 citation statements)
references
References 82 publications
12
278
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Even if a subject uses some counterexample reasoning, the mental cuing approach suggests generally better recall for "yes" responses than for "no" responses . However, some researchers have predicted equal recall of "yes" and "no" target items based on the organization of the list into "yes" and "no" categories in memory (Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986) . Yet, from the mental-euing point of view, this explanation is flawed because there are only two category cues in memory, representing the "yes" and "no" categories, and they have too many target items associated with them to be effective .…”
Section: Discussion the Congruent-infonnation Hypothesismentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Even if a subject uses some counterexample reasoning, the mental cuing approach suggests generally better recall for "yes" responses than for "no" responses . However, some researchers have predicted equal recall of "yes" and "no" target items based on the organization of the list into "yes" and "no" categories in memory (Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986) . Yet, from the mental-euing point of view, this explanation is flawed because there are only two category cues in memory, representing the "yes" and "no" categories, and they have too many target items associated with them to be effective .…”
Section: Discussion the Congruent-infonnation Hypothesismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In spite of this theoretical expectation, some researchers did not find superior recall for items judged as selfdescriptive (Kendzierski, 1980;Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986). In a review of relevant research, Higgins and Bargh (1987) concluded that the great majority of studies found…”
Section: Hypothesismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although many subsequent studies found superior recall following self-reference (SR; e.g., Bellezza, 1984;Kuiper & Rogers, 1979;Maki & McCaul, 1985), other research suggest that the SRE was not so robust. Specifically, other kinds of non-SR processing appeared to promote memory as well as or better than SR (e.g., Bellezza & Hoyt, 1992;Keenan & Baillet, 1980;Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986;Lord, 1980). These conflicting study findings led Higgins and Bargh (1987) to conclude that "self-reference is neither necessary nor suffi-cient for memory of input to be facilitated in comparison to a semantic orientation task" (p. 392).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These conflicting study findings led Higgins and Bargh (1987) to conclude that "self-reference is neither necessary nor suffi-cient for memory of input to be facilitated in comparison to a semantic orientation task" (p. 392). Moreover, after 2 decades, researchers seem divided between those who are willing to attribute the SRE to special mnemonic properties of the self (e.g., Maki & McCaul, 1985;Rogers et al, 1977) and those who are not (e.g., Brown, Keenan, & Potts, 1986;Klein & Kihlstrom, 1986).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation