1981
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.7.3.181
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Elaboration and distinctiveness in memory for faces.

Abstract: This research attempts to account for the finding that faces that have been judged with reference to traits such as honesty or friendliness are better remembered than faces judged with respect to a physical feature. Four experiments are reported in which the orienting task engaged in by the subjects was controlled. The first two experiments support an elaboration hypothesis that it is the amount rather than the type of information encoded that accounts for the observed effect. Experiments 3 and 4 provide evide… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

25
165
6
5

Year Published

2005
2005
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 163 publications
(201 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
25
165
6
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Visual-processing accounts argue that facilitation arises as a function of the quantity or quality of visual information encoded about the face. Facilitation may be due to a greater number of features being attended to and stored during encoding (the feature quantity account; e.g., Blaney & Winograd, 1978;Courtois & Mueller, 1979;Winograd, 1978Winograd, , 1981 or to the encoding of more holistic impressions of the face (i.e., the interrelations between features), in addition to feature-based information (the holistic account; e.g., Berman & Cutler, 1998;Wells & Hryciw, 1984). Alternatively, semantic-processing accounts suggest that facilitation may be due to the addition of semantic associations to the described face, which benefit retrieval (e.g., Anderson & Reder, 1979;Bruce & Young, 1986;Ryan & Schooler, 1994, cited in Schooler, Ryan, & Reder, 1996.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Visual-processing accounts argue that facilitation arises as a function of the quantity or quality of visual information encoded about the face. Facilitation may be due to a greater number of features being attended to and stored during encoding (the feature quantity account; e.g., Blaney & Winograd, 1978;Courtois & Mueller, 1979;Winograd, 1978Winograd, , 1981 or to the encoding of more holistic impressions of the face (i.e., the interrelations between features), in addition to feature-based information (the holistic account; e.g., Berman & Cutler, 1998;Wells & Hryciw, 1984). Alternatively, semantic-processing accounts suggest that facilitation may be due to the addition of semantic associations to the described face, which benefit retrieval (e.g., Anderson & Reder, 1979;Bruce & Young, 1986;Ryan & Schooler, 1994, cited in Schooler, Ryan, & Reder, 1996.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A teoria prediz que uma face típica, ou seja, uma face cujos elementos seriam comuns às faces já observadas, seria facilmente confundida com outras. Já uma face peculiar seria identificada de forma mais acurada (Barlett, Hurry, & Thorley, 1984;Winograd, 1981).…”
Section: Discussionunclassified
“…These results make sense because conscious memory is known lead to superior performance for arousing (Mather & Sunderland, 2011), uncommon (Winograd, 1981), and valenced (Hamann et al, 1999) stimuli.…”
Section: Conscious Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The GMP hypothesis states that location memory should be better for stimuli that are more arousing (Mather & Sunderland, 2011), less common, (Barclay, 2008;Winograd, 1981), and more strongly valenced (positively or negatively) (Hamann et al, 1999) than other stimuli. As discussed above, ancestral nonthreatening stimuli, ancestral threatening stimuli, and modern threatening stimuli were all rated as significantly more arousing than modern nonthreatening stimuli.…”
Section: Survival Processing Hypothesis This Hypothesis States That mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation