2003
DOI: 10.1080/10871200304306
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effort and the Functional Response of Deer Hunters

Abstract: Managers of overabundant deer have failed to incorporate relevant predatorprey theory into management research. In particular, understanding the functional response of deer hunters (deer encountered/time) to declining deer density is important because functional responses determine relative effort (time/deer encountered) required to harvest a deer and may, in turn, influence hunter perceptions of deer density and costs associated with deer removal. We used information-theoretic techniques and nonlinear regress… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

4
57
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(61 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
4
57
0
Order By: Relevance
“…S8). Analysis of the hunter functional response from 10 datasets supports a type I functional response (41), which suggests that hunter success rates are expected to increase linearly, rather than simply monotonically, with deer density. Additionally, hunter success rates ( Fig.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…S8). Analysis of the hunter functional response from 10 datasets supports a type I functional response (41), which suggests that hunter success rates are expected to increase linearly, rather than simply monotonically, with deer density. Additionally, hunter success rates ( Fig.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To meet constituency demands and improve harvest management, managers need to better understand how regulations influence hunter participation and behavior. Knowledge of hunter experience, persistence and dedication, and conservation concern may inform hunter recruitment and retention strategies (Enck et al, 1993;Miller & Vaske, 2003;Stedman et al, 2004;Van Deelen & Etter, 2003), addressing the current decline in waterfowl hunter numbers (Vrtiska, Gammonley, Naylor, & Raedeke, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although meat or other products, such as pelts, usually are derived from the hunting activities, the hunters are neither relying on these products for their subsistence (as subsistence hunters described above), nor are their chief interest to decimate game numbers (as persecution hunters). Because the motivation of these hunters is related to the expected positive experience of the hunt, which usually is related to the likelihood of seeing or killing game (Gigliotti, 2000;Dickson, Hutton and Adams, 2009), we would expect these hunters to exhibit a threshold in game abundances below which the expected likelihood of killing game would be too low to warrant the financial or time investment of the hunt (Van Deelen and Etter, 2003). Because of this motivational characteristic, we would predict that recreational hunters would behave like generalist predators, and not hunt game when they fall below a certain threshold in abundance.…”
Section: Motivational Drivers Behind Huntingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These predation strategies have direct ramifications for hunting, since they are predicted to have different potential to influence prey populations (e.g., Anderson and Erlinge, 1977). However, while the predation strategies exhibited by non-human predators have evolved to maximize fitness, human hunting strategies are also determined by financial, emotional, social, and cultural factors (Van Deelen and Etter, 2003). Therefore, the relationship between human hunting and game abundance may be more complex than what predator-prey models would predict (Heberlein and Kuentzel, 2002).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%