1996
DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1164
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of suspicion on attributional thinking and the correspondence bias.

Abstract: This research examined why suspicion of ulterior motives leads perceivers to avoid the correspondence bias in the assigned-essay paradigm, in contrast to information about situational constraint. Five experiments offer converging evidence that suspicion triggers active, sophisticated attributional thinking. These studies examined participants' spontaneous thoughts and attributional analyses in the context of high-constraint or ulterior-motives conditions. The studies (a) suggest that high-constraint informatio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

12
294
1
7

Year Published

2008
2008
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 329 publications
(326 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
12
294
1
7
Order By: Relevance
“…Perhaps, the most popular view is that observers first identify and categorize a person's behavior (e.g., helpful gesture) and then attribute the corresponding trait to the actor [e.g., he or she is helpful; Gilbert and Malone, 1995;Trope and Gaunt, 2000]. A related perspective is that we first identify the intentions, desires, or motives of an actor spontaneously [Fein, 1996;Hassin et al, 2005;Heider, 1958;Malle, 1999;Read and Miller, 1993] and that this process shapes the trait inferences we subsequently make. For instance, when an actor engages in helpful behavior, we wonder which reasons or motives may have compelled the actor to do so (out of sincere desire to help or to ingratiate?)…”
Section: Theoretical Perspectives On Social Cognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Perhaps, the most popular view is that observers first identify and categorize a person's behavior (e.g., helpful gesture) and then attribute the corresponding trait to the actor [e.g., he or she is helpful; Gilbert and Malone, 1995;Trope and Gaunt, 2000]. A related perspective is that we first identify the intentions, desires, or motives of an actor spontaneously [Fein, 1996;Hassin et al, 2005;Heider, 1958;Malle, 1999;Read and Miller, 1993] and that this process shapes the trait inferences we subsequently make. For instance, when an actor engages in helpful behavior, we wonder which reasons or motives may have compelled the actor to do so (out of sincere desire to help or to ingratiate?)…”
Section: Theoretical Perspectives On Social Cognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To do so, they develop knowledge structures about the motives of advertisers and the tactics used in advertising, which help them respond to advertising in a way that suits their own goals (Campbell and Kirmani 2000;Friestad and Wright 1994). In particular, consumers use their prior experience and knowledge of marketing tactics to make inferences about the manipulative intent, deceptiveness, and ulterior motives of advertisers (Campbell 1995;Darke and Ritchie 2007;Fein 1996). We suggest that consumers may believe that fast disclaimers represent a tactic used by advertisers to hide information or to deceive consumers.…”
Section: Advertisement Disclaimers and Speed Of Communicationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In situations of low trustworthiness or suspicion, performance may be suspect. Fein (1996) found that possibilities of deception or suspicious behavior might lead individuals to be more cautious (and perhaps more careful) in decision making thus slowing down their judgment processes. This has both positive and negative consequences.…”
Section: Performance In Hierarchical Organizations Is Independent Of mentioning
confidence: 99%