2015
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-015-0875-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of stimulus order on discrimination sensitivity for short and long durations

Abstract: Previous studies have shown that discrimination sensitivity in 2AFC tasks depends on the presentation order of the standard and comparison stimulus. The present study examined whether this so-called Type B effect generalizes across different standard magnitudes. Therefore, Experiment 1 employed an auditory duration discrimination task with short (100 ms) and long (1,000 ms) standard durations and a constant interstimulus interval (ISI) of 1,000 ms. For both standard durations, a clear Type B effect emerged. In… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
26
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
26
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…One exception is Hellström and Rammsayer (2004), who, using a 50-ms standard and interstimulus intervals of 100 through 2,700 ms, obtained consistently positive SPEs. Another exception is the recent study by Bausenhart, Dyjas, and Ulrich (2015), using an adaptive method, in which negative SPEs were found for filled auditory intervals with 100-ms and 1,000-ms standards and an interstimulus interval of 1,000 ms. With a 100ms standard, the SPE vanished when the interstimulus interval was shortened to 300 ms, but it did not reverse into positive except for a few participants.…”
mentioning
confidence: 83%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…One exception is Hellström and Rammsayer (2004), who, using a 50-ms standard and interstimulus intervals of 100 through 2,700 ms, obtained consistently positive SPEs. Another exception is the recent study by Bausenhart, Dyjas, and Ulrich (2015), using an adaptive method, in which negative SPEs were found for filled auditory intervals with 100-ms and 1,000-ms standards and an interstimulus interval of 1,000 ms. With a 100ms standard, the SPE vanished when the interstimulus interval was shortened to 300 ms, but it did not reverse into positive except for a few participants.…”
mentioning
confidence: 83%
“…With this kind of model, the authors account for the Bcontraction bias^in their experimentthat is, TOEs that are positive for low, and negative for high, magnitudes. (In their experiment, unlike those of Bausenhart et al 2015, both stimuli were varied, which made the TOEs visible.) This explanation may be plausible in most experimental conditions, but not in those of Hellström (1979Hellström ( , 2003 with brief, but by no means extreme, stimulus durations and interstimulus intervals.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Moreover, using a roving or a reminder method also has an impact on duration discrimination: Discrimination is better with the reminder method-that is, with the standard interval kept constant in the first position (Grondin & McAuley 2009;Hellström & Rammsayer 2004). The time-order effect is sometimes referred to as a Type-A effect, and the fact of having better performances in the standard-comparison order than in the comparison-standard order is sometimes referred to as a Type-B effect (Bausenhart, Dyjas, & Ulrich, 2015), or standard position effect (Hellström & Rammsayer 2015). Time-order errors tend to be negative for relatively short durations-that is, the second duration is judged as longer than the first-but tend to be positive for relatively longer durations, especially those judged in retrospect-that is, the first duration is judged longer than the second (Block 1985).…”
Section: 21mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another example of how prior knowledge can affect behavioral performance from the time perception literature is the work of Dyjas, Bausenhart, and Ulrich (2014) on stimulus order effects in interval comparison tasks. Typically, discrimination performance is lower if the comparison interval precedes the standard interval (Bausenhart, Dyjas, & Ulrich, 2015;Lapid, Ulrich, & Rammsayer, 2008). However, when participants are informed about the stimulus order before a trial starts, this effect is greatly reduced suggesting that participants have, to a certain degree, top-down control on the processing of intervals .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%