2001
DOI: 10.1901/jeab.2001.75-165
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of Signaled Versus Unsignaled Delay of Reinforcement on Choice

Abstract: Pigeons chose between 5-s and 15-s delay-of-reinforcement alternatives. The first key peck to satisfy the choice schedule began a delay timer, and food was delivered at the end of the interval. Key pecks during the delay interval were measured, but had no scheduled effect. In Experiment 1, signal conditions and choice schedules were varied across conditions. During unsignaled conditions, no stimulus change signaled the beginning of a delay interval. During differential and nondifferential signal conditions, of… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4

Citation Types

1
17
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
(45 reference statements)
1
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Mazur 1987Mazur , 1997Gibbon et al 1988;Grace 1994;Richards 1997Richards , 1999Mobini et al 2000). However, the precise role of such stimuli in influencing choice behaviour remains controversial (Mazur 1995(Mazur , 1997McDevitt and Williams 2001). According to Mazur (1995Mazur ( , 1997, intra-delay stimuli acquire conditioned reinforcing properties by virtue of their association with the delayed primary reinforcers, the reinforcing efficacy of such stimuli being inversely related to the time spent in their presence.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Mazur 1987Mazur , 1997Gibbon et al 1988;Grace 1994;Richards 1997Richards , 1999Mobini et al 2000). However, the precise role of such stimuli in influencing choice behaviour remains controversial (Mazur 1995(Mazur , 1997McDevitt and Williams 2001). According to Mazur (1995Mazur ( , 1997, intra-delay stimuli acquire conditioned reinforcing properties by virtue of their association with the delayed primary reinforcers, the reinforcing efficacy of such stimuli being inversely related to the time spent in their presence.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When food is delayed by blackout in concurrent schedules (Chung & Herrnstein, 1967), the procedure is similar to concurrent-chain schedules, the concurrent situation being like the initial link and the blackouts being like the terminal links (Davison, 1983). Much research has addressed the effects of delay on extended concurrent-and concurrent-chain-schedule performance, both using food delayed in blackout and food available after responding on distinctively signaled schedules (i.e., standard concurrent-chain schedules; see McDevitt &Williams, 2001, andOmino &Ito, 1993, for a comparison of these procedures). Although the length of the initial links of concurrent chains affects choice a little when food delivery in the terminal links is immediate (i.e., concurrent schedules; Alsop & Elliffe, 1988;Elliffe & Alsop, 1996), the length of the initiallink schedules (i.e., reciprocal of terminal-link entry rate) affects choice strongly when food is delayed (Berg & Grace, 2004).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, in the present experiment, signals facilitated efficient responding, consistent with previous literature on the sunk-cost effect and other areas of inquiry (cf. Lieving, Reilly & Lattal, 2006;McDevitt & Williams, 2001;Navarro & Fantino, 2005, 2007Siegel & Rachlin, 1995).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Ainslie, 1975;Diller, Saunders & Anderson, 2008;Mazur, 1987) and in other areas as well (cf. Beardsley & Balster, 1993;Lattal, 1984;McDevitt, & Williams, 2001;Stretch, Gerber & Lane, 1976). Increasing the delay to the onset of the next trial should decrease responding that is followed by the delay.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation