2008
DOI: 10.1037/a0012659
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of scenery, lighting, glideslope, and experience on timing the landing flare.

Abstract: This study examined three visual strategies for timing the initiation of the landing flare based on perceptions of either: (a) a critical height above ground level; (b) a critical runway width angle (Psi); or (c) a critical time-to-contact (TTC) with the runway. Visual displays simulated landing approaches with trial-to-trial variations in glideslope, lighting, and scene detail. Twenty-four participants (8 private pilots, 8 student pilots, and 8 nonpilots) were instructed to initiate the flare when they percei… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Evidence suggests that the TTC, or optical variable tau that describes the relative velocity of an optical image expanding across the retina, can be registered directly by the human eye [10][11][12]. Normally, pilots obtain many (visual) cues to adopt a correct timing and control strategy.…”
Section: Flare Initiation Strategiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Evidence suggests that the TTC, or optical variable tau that describes the relative velocity of an optical image expanding across the retina, can be registered directly by the human eye [10][11][12]. Normally, pilots obtain many (visual) cues to adopt a correct timing and control strategy.…”
Section: Flare Initiation Strategiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, it is an important factor, especially in the sagittal plane, for pilots landing an aircraft. Optic flow cues such as the focus of expansion, the point from which the movement of all points in the field expand out during linear self-motion, are useful when landing an aircraft (Gibson et al 1955 ) and for determining the timing of the ‘flare’, the point where a pilot ‘levels out’ to avoid impacting the ground (Palmisano et al 2008 ). Studies show that humans are less accurate at judging vertical heading with discrimination thresholds of 2.5°–3° (Palmisano and Gillam 2005 ) and also less precise (Palmisano and Gillam 2005 ; MacNeilage et al 2010 ) than horizontal heading.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Since previous research on landing approaches has focused on already-trained participants (Palmisano and Gillam 2005 ; Gibb et al 2008 ; Kim et al 2010 ), humans’ innate ability to gauge approach angles, i.e., vertical heading direction in the sagittal plane, and the effect of training on such judgements, is still unknown. During training, pilots are taught to use the focus of expansion as their ‘aim-point’ (Palmisano et al 2008 ) and to use this point to gauge their rate of descent. Closely following Palmisano and Gillam’s ( 2005 ) vertical heading detection threshold task, we measured people’s baseline vertical heading judgement ability prior to providing training on a flight simulator program.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A manually executed flare is therefore by nature a multivariable control task that requires precise coupling between timing (initiation) and action (force on the control(s)) [10,11]. The coupling between timing and action increases the task difficulty, but it has as benefit the provided trained operators with a compensation option.…”
Section: The Flare Maneuvermentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Evidence suggests that the TTC, or optical variable tau that describes the relative velocity of an optical image expanding across the retina, can be registered directly by the human eye [10][11][12]. Normally, pilots obtain many (visual) cues to adopt a correct timing and control strategy.…”
Section: Flare Initiation Strategiesmentioning
confidence: 99%