1994
DOI: 10.1007/bf02168075
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of reward and nonreward on frustration and attention in attention deficit disorder

Abstract: The effects of reward schedule (100%, 50%, and 30%) and termination of rewards (extinction) on 30 attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity (ADD-H) and 30 normal children were studied using measures of frustration (speed/strength of lever pulling) and attention (reaction time to a light signal). ADD-Hs pulled harder on the lever than controls during extinction and on the lowest (30%) partial schedule, providing empirical evidence that they respond with greater frustration than normals when expected rewards… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
120
2
3

Year Published

2000
2000
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 145 publications
(132 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
6
120
2
3
Order By: Relevance
“…In the incentive first group, the no-incentive block may be subjectively experienced as a form of punishment since children were initially rewarded for certain behaviors that they subsequently were asked to perform without reward. According to Gray (1982) and others (Corr 2002;Douglas and Parry 1994), removal of reward (i.e., extinction) is essentially equivalent to punishment. Thus, the decline in performance in children with ADHD when rewards are omitted may reflect frustration, either due to excessive sensitivity to punishment or reward, or possibly an interaction between these motivational systems postulated by Gray (Corr 2002;Gray 1982;see Quay 1988, for a discussion of motivational systems in disruptive behavior disorders).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In the incentive first group, the no-incentive block may be subjectively experienced as a form of punishment since children were initially rewarded for certain behaviors that they subsequently were asked to perform without reward. According to Gray (1982) and others (Corr 2002;Douglas and Parry 1994), removal of reward (i.e., extinction) is essentially equivalent to punishment. Thus, the decline in performance in children with ADHD when rewards are omitted may reflect frustration, either due to excessive sensitivity to punishment or reward, or possibly an interaction between these motivational systems postulated by Gray (Corr 2002;Gray 1982;see Quay 1988, for a discussion of motivational systems in disruptive behavior disorders).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Participants were randomly assigned to receive either the incentive or no-incentive condition first since the order in which the incentives are presented may influence performance (Corr 2002;Douglas and Parry 1994). Table 1 presents sample characteristics for each incentive order.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Similarly, the generalization of the new skills to other contexts requires their reinforcement across different settings in the child' s natural environment. Several studies suggest that ADHD children are overly sensitive to the presence and removal of reinforcements (for example, Douglas and Parry, 1994;Tripp and Alsop, 1999). These studies underscore the importance of long-term reinforcement for this population; in particular, the reinforcement should work best if it is administered on a dense, consistent, and continuous schedule.…”
Section: Social Skills Interventionsmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…This impulsivity leads to several detrimental effects: Children with ADHD show risky behavior (i.e., fast driving; Jerome et al 2006), sensation seeking (Antrop et al 2000), are responsive to immediate rewards (Douglas and Parry 1994), and are less able to delay gratification or resist temptation .…”
Section: Self-control In Children With Adhdmentioning
confidence: 99%