2004
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2004.00257.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of Recasts on the Acquisition of the Aspectual Form ‐te i‐(ru) by Learners of Japanese as a Foreign Language

Abstract: University of Hawai'i at M a anoaThe present study investigated the effects of intensive recasting on second language learners' use of the Japanese aspectual form -te i-(ru) using a time-series design. Four college classroom learners participated in 8 conversational sessions, with the researcher providing recasts during the middle 4 sessions, and 2 of the learners also participated in a delayed-posttest session after 7 weeks. Overall accuracy increased significantly in correlation with the number of recasts pr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
101
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 86 publications
(102 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
1
101
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These laboratory studies have generally yielded positive results for recasts, but many have methodological problems-such as no control group (e.g., Ishida, 2004 ) or a control group receiving no CF (e.g., Han, 2002 ;McDonough & Mackey, 2006 ;Sagarra 2007 )-as well as other limitations, as we will point out, akin to those that Goo and Mackey associate only with the alleged "case against recasts." Whereas the lab studies to which Goo and Mackey appeal for support fall short of substantiating their claim for across-the-board benefi ts of recasts, the classroom studies to which they refer (i.e., Doughty & Varela, 1998 ;Goo, 2012 ;Loewen & Nabei, 2007 ) provide even less support, as we explain next.…”
Section: Are Recasts Effective Across the Board?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These laboratory studies have generally yielded positive results for recasts, but many have methodological problems-such as no control group (e.g., Ishida, 2004 ) or a control group receiving no CF (e.g., Han, 2002 ;McDonough & Mackey, 2006 ;Sagarra 2007 )-as well as other limitations, as we will point out, akin to those that Goo and Mackey associate only with the alleged "case against recasts." Whereas the lab studies to which Goo and Mackey appeal for support fall short of substantiating their claim for across-the-board benefi ts of recasts, the classroom studies to which they refer (i.e., Doughty & Varela, 1998 ;Goo, 2012 ;Loewen & Nabei, 2007 ) provide even less support, as we explain next.…”
Section: Are Recasts Effective Across the Board?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, it is in contrast with Yang and Lyster's (2010) study in which they believe that learners benefits more from prompts than from recasts. Laboratory studies have generally yielded positive results for recasts, but not necessarily in comparison with other clearly defined types of feedbacks (e.g., Han, 2002;Ishida, 2004;Iwashita, 2003;Long et al, 1998;Mackey & Philp, 1998;McDonough & Mackey, 2006). Leeman's (2003) study, however, which included a comparison of recasts and repetition of error without any opportunities for immediate repair, showed both feedback types to be equally effective.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For the same reason, Ishida (2004) studied the impacts of recasting on the acquisition of Japanese aspectual form -te i (ru), leading to the significantly increased overall accuracy in connection with the number of recasts which was given throughout the treatment period, and the accuracy rate was sustained. Ellis and sheen (2006) in their research entitled -Reexamining the role of recasts in second language acquisition‖ argued the probability of overestimation about acquisition usefulness of recasts compared with other corrective feedback types.…”
Section: Literature Reviewmentioning
confidence: 99%