1993
DOI: 10.1577/1548-8675(1993)013<0217:eorban>2.3.co;2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of Recall Bias and Nonresponse Bias on Self-Report Estimates of Angling Participation

Abstract: We addressed the problems of recall and nonresponse biases in self-report angling participation surveys. While each source of bias has been recognized as problematic, previous research has not addressed the interaction effects of these biases. Recall bias was assessed by comparing immediate, 3-month, and 6-month recall periods. A diary format was used for the immediate recall period and mail surveys for the 3-month and 6-month recall periods. Nonresponse bias was assessed by conducting telephone interviews wit… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
74
1
4

Year Published

2002
2002
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 103 publications
(83 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
2
74
1
4
Order By: Relevance
“…However, it is consistent with the number of participants in many VAD programs reported in the literature (e.g., Younk and Cook 1992;Tarrant et al 1993). Nonparticipants in this case were individuals who had at some point registered for the AAP but had never participated, and it is possible that the barriers to participation differ for those who had never interacted with the program at all.…”
Section: Barriersupporting
confidence: 85%
“…However, it is consistent with the number of participants in many VAD programs reported in the literature (e.g., Younk and Cook 1992;Tarrant et al 1993). Nonparticipants in this case were individuals who had at some point registered for the AAP but had never participated, and it is possible that the barriers to participation differ for those who had never interacted with the program at all.…”
Section: Barriersupporting
confidence: 85%
“…First, because angler app users may be study subjects in addition to data collectors, angler app data may be more prone to biases that result from non-random participation (i.e. App data are likely to suffer from avidity bias (Jiorle 2015) because the willingness of anglers to share data increases with avidity (Harris and Bergersen 1985;Tarrant et al 1993;Connelly and Brown 1995), and to underestimate effort if there is little incentive to log trips that do not result in a catch. For example, data from the fishing app iAngler (Table 1) were biased towards urbanized regions and inshore species (Jiorle 2015).…”
Section: Data Quality and Biasmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…can be subsampled according to the demographic(s) of interest or reweighted based on statistically rigorous estimates of the general angler population (e.g. Finally, persistent biases can be addressed via correction factors that are obtained through validation or simulation (for conventional examples seeTarrant et al 1993;Connelly and Brown 1995;Connelly et al 2000;Sullivan 2002;Ashford et al 2010). Finally, persistent biases can be addressed via correction factors that are obtained through validation or simulation (for conventional examples seeTarrant et al 1993;Connelly and Brown 1995;Connelly et al 2000;Sullivan 2002;Ashford et al 2010).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, catch rates may be a predictor of recreational demand, because catch may be perceived as a measure of site quality (Parsons and Needelman, 1992;Englin and Lambert, 1995). Furthermore, using self-reported catch as a demand predictor also leads to measurement error due to recall bias (Tarrant et al, 1993;Morey and Waldman, 1998). Structural equation models have been used to circumvent this issue.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%