1998
DOI: 10.1080/00220979809598343
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of Practicing Self-Monitoring of Mathematical Problem-Solving Heuristics on Impulsive and Reflective College Students' Heuristics Knowledge and Problem-Solving Ability

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

2000
2000
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In contrast, the good problem solvers were more likely to produce justifications after regulations, a pattern that converges with previous studies indicating the cognitive benefits of meta-regulatory activities (e.g., Lan, Repman, & Chyung, 1998;Schauble, 1990). For example, in the context of self-explanation, Chi et al ( 1989) found that successful learners more frequently elaborated on the application conditions and the goals of operators.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…In contrast, the good problem solvers were more likely to produce justifications after regulations, a pattern that converges with previous studies indicating the cognitive benefits of meta-regulatory activities (e.g., Lan, Repman, & Chyung, 1998;Schauble, 1990). For example, in the context of self-explanation, Chi et al ( 1989) found that successful learners more frequently elaborated on the application conditions and the goals of operators.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…Again, a limited informationprocessing capacity is probably the reason for the lack of metacognitive gains. According to Lan and Repman (1998) metacognitive instructions too demand an amount of cognitive capacities which may lead to a pernicious competition between metacognitive instructions and other learning processes. This is in line with the theory of utilisation deficiency in which children experience little or no benefit in task performance from the use of a particular strategy (Bjorklund, Miller, Coyle & Slawinsky, 1997).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Within the scientific thinking domain, research has repeatedly demonstrated that metacognitive support improves performance (Beishuizen, Wilhelm, & Schimmel, 2004;Lan & Repman, 1998;White & Frederiksen, 1998;Wilhelm, Beishuizen, & Van Rijn, 2005;Zion, Michalsky, & Mevarech, 2005). For instance, White and Frederiksen (1998) investigated whether students from grades 7 and 9 benefitted from learning the inquiry cycle.…”
Section: Figurementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Singer and Cauraugh (1985) and Zimmerman (1998) suggest that learners should limit self-monitoring to key processes and outcomes when performing complex tasks. Others (Lan et al, 1998;Winne, 1995) suggest that learners practice self-monitoring strategies to an automatic level, a process called automatization or routinization, to reduce the demand of the self-monitoring process on working memory capacity. Based on the findings of the present study, we would like to suggest another practical way to help students with self-monitoring when engaged in difficult tasks.…”
Section: Effects Of Task Importance On Use Of Self-monitoring Strategiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…By providing a protocol which helped graduate students to record the amount of time they spent in learning important concepts in a statistics course and monitor their understanding of the concepts, researchers (Lan, 1996;Lan, Bradley, & Parr, 1993) improved students' academic performance on course examinations and use of learning strategies in the class. Lan, Repman, and Chyung (1998) also found that displaying a diagram of a problem-solving procedure on computer screens or asking a series of questions to lead students through the procedure step-by-step improved college students' knowledge retention and problem-solving performance.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%