2017
DOI: 10.1037/bne0000192
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of inference on dopaminergic prediction errors depend on orbitofrontal processing.

Abstract: Dopaminergic reward prediction errors in monkeys reflect inferential reward predictions that well-trained animals can make when associative rules change. Here, in a new analysis of previously described data, we test whether dopaminergic error signals in rats are influenced by inferential predictions and whether such effects depend on the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). Dopamine neurons were recorded from controls or rats with ipsilateral OFC lesions during performance of a choice task in which odor cues signaled t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
19
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
1
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This fact indicates that OFC tracks multiple influences on choice, but does not integrate them into a coherent abstract value variable, and suggests that such integration into an abstract value variable, if it occurs, takes place in a downstream structure. (Note that a good deal of evidence supports the idea that the midbrain DA system is downstream of OFC [45,46,47]. More broadly, these results fit into theories about hierarchies in reward processing [48,49,50,51].…”
Section: Reward Areassupporting
confidence: 64%
“…This fact indicates that OFC tracks multiple influences on choice, but does not integrate them into a coherent abstract value variable, and suggests that such integration into an abstract value variable, if it occurs, takes place in a downstream structure. (Note that a good deal of evidence supports the idea that the midbrain DA system is downstream of OFC [45,46,47]. More broadly, these results fit into theories about hierarchies in reward processing [48,49,50,51].…”
Section: Reward Areassupporting
confidence: 64%
“…While the present analyses were focused on spatial navigation, predictive representations are generalizable to non-spatial domains as well. Examples include relational knowledge and category generalization (Constantinescu et al, 2016;Garvert et al, 2017) , abstraction and transfer (Cole et al, 2011) , reward predictions (Takahashi et al, 2017) , associative inference, and schema learning (Hebscher & Gilboa, 2016;McKenzie et al, 2014;Moscovitch & Melo, 1997;Spalding et al, 2018;van Kesteren et al, 2013;Yu, 2018;Zeithamova et al, 2012;Zeithamova & Preston, 2010) . Previous work has proposed a hierarchy of time-scales in the brain (Chen et al, 2015) and indicated a role for hippocampal-prefrontal interactions in integrating episodes to build abstract schema (Schlichting & Preston, 2017) .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Both electrophysiological [77] and lesion data [5] suggest that even without a functioning OFC, the striatum has access to a state representation that is bound to external stimuli ('observable states'; although unobservable information about timing is also likely computed in the striatum [85]). In contrast, inferred states that incorporate internal information from, e.g., working memory, intended actions, or future goals, and are based on latent-cause inference as discussed above ('partially observable states') seem to require the OFC [5,[86][87]. This can explain why decision making becomes more OFC-dependent as tasks rely more on inference processes necessary to determine the underlying hidden state of a task.…”
Section: The Neural Substrates Of Representation Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Error bars: SEM; dashed horizontal: chance baseline; * p<0.05 compared to chance, one-tailed. Figure adapted from [6] and [87].…”
Section: Open Questions: Representation Learning In Real Time In Thementioning
confidence: 99%