2019
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0214226
|View full text |Cite|
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of hazard types on drivers’ risk rating and hazard response in a video-based hazard perception task

Abstract: Hazard perception is crucial for identifying potential hazards on the road, and how quick drivers can respond to the hazard partially relies on their risk rating of the hazard after they detect it. Although many studies have attempted to reveal the relationship between drivers’ response latencies and their risk ratings, this relationship has not been extensively explored under different hazard types. The present study addresses this issue using a video-based hazard perception task. Forty novice drivers and 35 … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, the drivers’ response time to hazard was significantly shorter than that to no hazard, indicating that it was more difficult for participants to evaluate no hazard situations. It is possible that the images do not contain any anticipatory cues and might need additional cognitive resources to search for hazards [24]. Another possible explanation is that drivers might expect hazards in all video clips, but they were not told that half of the clips contained no hazards.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Moreover, the drivers’ response time to hazard was significantly shorter than that to no hazard, indicating that it was more difficult for participants to evaluate no hazard situations. It is possible that the images do not contain any anticipatory cues and might need additional cognitive resources to search for hazards [24]. Another possible explanation is that drivers might expect hazards in all video clips, but they were not told that half of the clips contained no hazards.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One hundred twenty images (Fig. 1) captured from a hazard perception test [24] were evaluated by two traffic psychology experts. Sixty images contained hazards (in the current traffic situation, drivers had to slow down or change driving lanes to avoid collisions), and 60 images contained no hazards (in the current traffic situation, drivers kept driving activities and did not need to change driving lanes).…”
Section: Experimental Materialsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Each clip included only one slowly developing hazard under varying circumstances situations (see Appendix B for a description of each clip). The appearance time and location of the hazard in each clip were random from one clip to another the hazard triggers in the video clips included cars, motorcycles, cyclists, road work and pedestrians, with the clip length ranging from 10 s to 16 s. The test demonstrated acceptable reliability in previous studies and effectively discriminated between the different driver groups with varied driving experience [ 28 , 29 ].…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…And 43% of drivers were overspeeding in the covert hazard scenarios. It indicates that drivers perceive a higher risk in covert hazard scenarios [45], which is a greater threat to driving safety. Indeed, the number of cases of collisions in overt hazard scenarios (1.9%) is found to be less than that in covert hazard scenarios (18.1%) through the analysis of collision accidents.…”
Section: Effects Of the Overt And Covert Hazard Scenarios On Drivers'...mentioning
confidence: 99%